Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2006 | 2 | 1 | 87-97

Article title

Perceptual latency priming and illusory line motion: Facilitation by gradients of attention?

Selected contents from this journal

Title variants

Languages of publication

Abstracts

EN
The phenomena of illusory line motion and perceptual latency priming are both assumed to reflect a facilitation of perceptual latency. The explanation of illusory line motion presupposes that attention is distributed in a gradient fashion whereas this is not a necessary part of the explanation of perceptual latency priming. Two experiments test whether an attentional gradient is present in perceptual latency priming. Evidence for a gradient was found within 2.5° of visual angle around the attended location, but not at a distance of 5° and more.

Year

Volume

2

Issue

1

Pages

87-97

Physical description

Contributors

  • Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
  • Department of Psychology, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany

References

  • Ansorge, U. (2004). Top-down contingencies of nonconscious priming revealed by dual-task interference.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,57A, 1123-1148.
  • Ansorge, U., & Heumann, M. (2006). Shifts of visuospatial attention to invisible (metacontrast-masked) singletons: Clues from reaction times and event-related potentials.Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 61-76. ACP
  • Ansorge, U., & Neumann, O. (2005). Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: Evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cueing task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 762-777.
  • Bessel, F. W. (1838). Untersuchungen über die Wahrscheinlichkeit der Beobachtungsfehler (Studies on the probability of observational errors).Astronomische Nachrichten (Astronomical News), 15, 369-404.
  • Boring, E. G. (1929).A history of experimental psychology.New York: Appleton-Century.
  • Downing, C. J. (1988). Expectancy and visual-spatial attention: Effects on perceptual quality.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 188-202.
  • Downing, P., & Treisman, A. (1997). The line-motion illusion: Attention or impletion?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 768-779.
  • Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1972). Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays.Perception & Psychophysics, 12, 201-204.
  • Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). GPOWER:A prior, posthoc, and compromise power analyses for MS-DOS[Computer program]. Bonn, FRG: Bonn University, Dep. of Psychology.
  • Finney, D. J. (1971).Probit analysis(3rd ed.). Cambridge: University Press.
  • Goolkasian, P. A., & Tarantino, M. (1999). Covert and overt attention and the processing of cues for location and target identification.Journal of General Psychology, 126, 235-260.
  • Grünau, M. v., Racette, L., & Kwas, M. (1996). Measuring the attentional speed-up in the motion induction effect.Vision Research, 36, 2433-2446.
  • Grünau, M. v., Saikali, Z., & Faubert, J. (1995). Processing speed in the motion induction effect.Perception, 24, 477-490.
  • Hamm, J. P., & Klein, R. M. (2002). Does attention follow the motion in the "shooting line" illusion?Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 279-291.
  • Handy, T. C., Kingstone, A., & Mangun, G. R. (1996). Spatial distribution of visual attention: Perceptual sensitivity and response latency.Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 613-627.
  • Hawkins, H. L., Hillyard, S. A., Luck, S. J., & Mouloua, M. (1990). Visual attention modulates signal detectability.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 802-811.
  • Henderson, J. M., & Macquistan, A. D. (1993). The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue.Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 221-230.
  • Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., & Shimojo, S. (1993a). Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation.Vision Research, 45, 1219-1240.
  • Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., & Shimojo, S. (1993b). Visual attention revealed by an illusion of motion.Neuroscience Research, 18, 11-18.
  • Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., & Shimojo, S. (1993c). Voluntary and stimulus-induced attention detected as motion sensation.Perception, 22, 517-526.
  • Hodgson, T. L., Müller, H. J., & O'Leary, M. J. (1999). Attentional localization prior to simple and directed manual responses.Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 308-321.
  • Hughes, H. C., & Zimba, L. D. (1985). Spatial maps of directed visual attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 409-430.
  • Hughes, H. C., & Zimba, L. D. (1987). Natural boundaries for the spatial spread of directed visual attention.Neuropsychologia, 25, 5-18.
  • Jancke, D., Chavane, F., Naaman, S., & Grinvald, A. (2004). Imaging cortical correlates of illusion in early visual cortex.Nature, 428, 423-426.
  • Jaśkowski, P., Skalska, B., & Verleger, R. (2003). How the self controls its "automatic pilot" when processing subliminal information.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 911-920.
  • Jaśkowski, P., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Schlotterbeck, E., & Verleger, R. (2002). Traces left on visual selective attention by stimuli that are not consciously identified.Psychological Science, 13, 48-54.
  • Karnath, H. O., Zimmer, U., & Lewald, J. (2002). Impaired perception of temporal order in auditory extinction.Neuropsychologia, 40, 1977-1982.
  • Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., Pohl, C., & Hoffmann, J. (2006). Priming for novel masked stimuli depends on target set size.Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 37-45. ACP
  • Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 976-992.
  • LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371-379.
  • Lleras, A., & Enns, J. (2004). Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 475-493.
  • Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1988). Spatial gradients of visual attention: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence.Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 70, 417-428.
  • McCormick, P. A. (1997). Orienting attention without awareness.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 168-180.
  • McCormick, P. A., & Klein, R. (1990). The spatial distribution of attention during covert visual orienting.Acta Psychologica, 75, 225-242.
  • Müller, H. J., & Findlay, J. M. (1987). Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention.Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 383-399.
  • Müller N. G., Mollenhauer, M., Rösler, A., & Kleinschmidt, A. (2005). The attentional field has a Mexican hat distribution.Vision Research45, 1129-1137.
  • Neumann, O. (1982). Experimente zum Fehrer-Raab-Effekt und das "Wetterwart"-Modell der visuellen Maskierung. [Experiments on the Fehrer-Raab effect and the "Weather Station" Model of visual masking.]Report No. 24/1982, Department of Psychology at the Ruhr-University of Bochum, Cognitive Psychology Unit.
  • Neumann, O., Esselmann, U., & Klotz, W. (1993). Differential effects of visual-spatial attention on response latency and temporal order judgment.Psychological Research, 56, 26-34.
  • Pashler, H. (1998).The psychology of attention.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3-25.
  • Robertson, I. H., Mattingley, J. B., Rorden, C., & Driver, J. (1998). Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness.Nature, 395, 169-173.
  • Rorden, C., Mattingley, J. B., Karnath, H.-O., & Driver, J. (1997). Visual extinction and prior entry: Impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal damage.Neuropsychologica, 35, 421-433.
  • Sanford, E. C. (1888). Personal equation.The American Journal of Psychology, 2, 3-38, 271-298, 403-430.
  • Scharlau, I. (2002). Leading, but not trailing, primes influence temporal order perception: Further evidence for an attentional account of perceptual latency priming.Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 1346-1360.
  • Scharlau, I. (2004a). Evidence against a response bias in temporal order judgments with attention manipulation by masked primes.Psychological Research, 68, 224-236.
  • Scharlau, I. (2004b). Illusory line motion and perceptual latency priming: Two alternative measures for attentional facilitation. In R. P. Würtz, & M. Lappe (Eds.),Dynamic Perception 2004: Workshop of GI section 1.0.4"Image Understanding" and the European Networks MUHCI and ECOVISION; Tübingen University, November 2004 (127-133). Berlin, Amsterdam: AKA IOS Press.
  • Scharlau, I. (2004c). Spatial distribution of visual attention in perceptual latency priming.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1411-1437.
  • Scharlau, I. (2005). Perceptual latency priming: A measure of attentional facilitation.Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Scharlau, I., & Ansorge, U. (2003). Direct parameter specification of an attention shift: Evidence from perceptual latency priming.Vision Research, 43, 1351-1363.
  • Scharlau, I., & Neumann, O. (2003a). Perceptual latency priming by masked and unmasked stimuli: Evidence for an attentional explanation.Psychological Research, 67, 184-197.
  • Scharlau, I., & Neumann, O. (2003b). Temporal parameters and time course of perceptual latency priming.Acta Psychologica, 113, 185-203.
  • Schlaghecken, F., & Sisman, R. (2006). Intact lowlevel motor inhibition in children: Evidence from the negative compatibility effect.Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 7-19. ACP
  • Schmidt, T. (2002). The finger in flight: Real-time motor control by visually masked color stimuli.Psychological Science, 13, 112-117.
  • Shimojo, S., Hikosaka, O., & Miyauchi, S. (1999). Automatic and controlled attention detected by the line motion effect. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.),Attention and performance XVII(pp. 145-163). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Shore, D. I., Spence, C., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry.Psychological Science, 12, 205-212.
  • Skalska, B., Jaśkowski, P., & van der Lubbe, R. (2006). Direct parameter specification and visuo-spatial attention in subliminal priming of motor reaction.Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 47-59. ACP
  • Steglich, C., & Neumann, O. (2000). Temporal, but not spatial, context modulates a masked prime's effect on temporal order judgment, but not on response latency.Psychological Research, 63, 36-47.
  • Stelmach, L. B., Campsall, J. M., & Herdman, C. M. (1997). Attentional and ocular movements.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 823-844.
  • Stelmach, L. B., & Herdman, C. M. (1991). Directed attention and perception of temporal order.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 539-550.
  • Titchener, E. M. (1908).Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention.New York: MacMillan.
  • Treisman, A. M. (1988). Features and objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,40A, 201-237.
  • Turatto, M., & Galfano, G. (2001). Attentional capture by color without any relevant attentional set.Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 286-297.
  • Ulrich, R. (1987). Threshold models of temporal order judgments evaluated by a ternary response task.Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 224-239.
  • Verleger, R., Jaśkowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der Lubbe, R., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative differences between conscious and nonconscious processing? On inverse priming induced by masked arrows.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 494-515.
  • Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 100, 6275-6280.
  • Wundt, W. (1887).Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie (Main features of physiological psychology).Leipzig: Engelmann.
  • Zackon, D. H., Casson, E. J., Zafar, A., Stelmach, L., & Racette, L. (1999). The temporal order judgment paradigm: Subcortical attentional contribution under exogenous and endogenous cueing conditions.Neuropsychologia, 37, 511-520.
  • Zimba, L. D., & Hughes, H. C. (1987). Distractor-target interactions during directed visual attention.Spatial Vision, 2, 117-149.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0047-7
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.