Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2009 | 5 | 27-41

Article title

Flexibility of temporal expectations for triple subdivision of a beat

Selected contents from this journal

Title variants

Languages of publication

Abstracts

EN
When tapping in synchrony with an isochronous sequence of beats, participants respond automatically to an unexpectedly early or late beat by shifting their next tap; this is termed thephase correction response(PCR). A PCR has also been observed in response to unexpected perturbations of metrical subdivisions of a beat, which suggests that participants have temporal expectancies for subdivisions to occur at particular time points. It has been demonstrated that a latent temporal expectancy at 1/2 of the inter-beat interval (IBI) exists even in the absence of explicit duple subdivision in previous IBIs of a sequence. The present study asked whether latent expectancies at 1/3 and 2/3 of the IBI can be induced by a global experimental context of triple subdivision, and whether a local context of consistently phase-shifted triple subdivisions can induce different expectancies. Using the PCR as the dependent variable, we find weak evidence for latent expectancies but strong evidence for context-induced shifts in expectancies. These results suggest that temporal referents between beats, which typically are linked to simple ratios of time spans, are flexible and context-dependent. In addition, we show that the PCR, a response to expectancy violation, is independent of and sometimes contrary to the simultaneous phase adaptation required by a change in subdivision timing.

Year

Volume

5

Pages

27-41

Physical description

Contributors

author
  • Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut
  • Cognitive Science Program, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut

References

  • Bergeson, T. R., & Trehub, S. E. (2006). Infants' perception of rhythmic patterns.Music Perception, 23, 345-360.
  • Bregman, A. S. (1990).Auditory scene analysis.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Collier, G. L., & Wright, C. E. (1995). Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 602-627.
  • Desain, P. (1992). A (de)composable theory of rhythm perception.Music Perception, 9, 439-454.
  • Drake, C. (1997). Motor and perceptually preferred synchronization by children and adults: Binary and ternary ratios.Polish Quarterly of Developmental Psychology, 3, 43-61.
  • Drake, C., & Bertrand, D. (2001). The quest for universals in temporal processing in music.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 17-27.
  • Friberg, A., & Sundström, A. (2002). Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance: Evidence for a common rhythmic pattern.Music Perception, 19, 333-349.
  • Honing, H., & de Haas, W. B. (2008). Swing once more: Related timing and tempo in expert jazz drumming.Music Perception, 25, 471-476.
  • Hove, M. J., Keller, P. E., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2007). Sensorimotor synchronization with chords containing tone-onset asynchronies.Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 699-708.
  • Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time.Psychological Review, 96, 459-491.
  • Large, E. W. (2000). On synchronizing movements to music.Human Movement Science, 19, 527-566.
  • Large, E. W., Fink, P., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). Tracking simple and complex sequences.Psychological Research, 66, 3-17.
  • Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events.Psychological Review, 106, 119-159.
  • Large, E. W., & Kolen, J. F. (1994). Resonance and the perception of musical meter.Connection Science, 6, 177-208.
  • London, J. (2004).Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of musical meter.New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Mates, J. (1994a). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. I. Timing and error corrections.Biological Cybernetics, 70, 463-473.
  • Mates, J. (1994b). A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence. II. Stability analysis, error estimation, and simulations.Biological Cybernetics, 70, 475-484.
  • Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3-18.
  • Pressing, J. (1998). Error correction processes in temporal pattern production.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 63-101.
  • Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization.Psychological Research, 63, 106-128.
  • Repp, B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 600-621.
  • Repp, B. H. (2002a). Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 410-430.
  • Repp, B. H. (2002b). Phase correction in sensorimotor synchronization: Nonlinearities in voluntary and involuntary responses to perturbations.Human Movement Science, 21, 1-37.
  • Repp, B. H. (2003a). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision.Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 355-370.
  • Repp, B. H. (2003b). Phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory sequences: Effects of single and periodic distractors on synchronization accuracy.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 290-309.
  • Repp, B. H. (2004). On the nature of phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with interleaved auditory sequences.Human Movement Science, 23, 389-413.
  • Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969-992.
  • Repp, B. H. (2006). Does an auditory perceptual illusion affect online auditory action control? The case of (de)accentuation and synchronization.Experimental Brain Research, 168, 493-504.
  • Repp, B. H. (2008a). Multiple temporal references in sensorimotor synchronization with metrical auditory sequences.Psychological Research, 72, 79-98.
  • Repp, B. H. (2008b). Metrical subdivision results in subjective slowing of the beat.Music Perception, 26, 19-39.
  • Repp, B. H. (2009). Segregated in perception, integrated for action: Immunity of rhythmic sensorimotor coordination to auditory stream segregation.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 426-434.
  • Sadakata, M., Desain, P., & Honing, H. (2006). The Bayesian way to relate rhythm perception and production.Music Perception, 23, 269-288.
  • Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2001). The coupled oscillator model of between-hand coordination in alternate-hand tapping: A reappraisal.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 251-265.
  • Sternberg, S., Knoll, R. L., & Zukofsky, P. (1982). Timing by skilled musicians. In D. Deutsch (Ed.),The psychology of music(pp. 181-239). New York: Academic Press.
  • Tomic, S. T., & Janata, P. (2008). Beyond the beat: Modeling metric structure in music and performance.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124, 4024-4041.
  • Vorberg, D., & Schulze, H. H. (2002). Linear phase-correction in synchronization: Predictions, parameter estimation, and simulations.Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 56-87.
  • Wohlschläger, A., & Koch, R. (2000). Synchronization error: An error in time perception. In P. Desain & L. Windsor (Eds.),Rhythm perception and production(pp. 115-128). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0063-7
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.