Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2010 | 6 | 1-14

Article title

Perceptual judgement and saccadic behaviour in a spatial distortion with briefly presented stimuli

Selected contents from this journal

Title variants

Languages of publication

Abstracts

EN
When observers are asked to localize the peripheral position of a small probe with respect to the mid-position of a spatially extended comparison stimulus, they tend to judge the probe as being more peripheral than the mid-position of the comparison stimulus. This relative mislocalization seems to emerge from differences in absolute localization, that is the comparison stimulus is localized more towards the fovea than the probe. The present study compared saccadic behaviour and relative localization judgements in three experiments and determined the quantitative relationship between both measures. The results showed corresponding effects in localization errors and saccadic behaviour. Moreover, it was possible to estimate the amount of the relative mislocalization by means of the saccadic amplitude.

Year

Volume

6

Pages

1-14

Physical description

Contributors

author
  • Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Germany
  • Department of Psychology, RWTH Aachen University, Germany
  • Faculteit der Sociale Wetenschappen, Instituut Psychologie, Leiden University, The Netherlands

References

  • Abrams, R. A., Meyer, D. E., & Kornblum, S. (1989). Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: Characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 529-543.
  • Aitsebaomo, A. P., & Bedell, H. E. (1992). Psychophysical and saccadic information about direction for briefly presented visual targets.Vision Research, 32, 1729-1737.
  • Awater, H., & Lappe, M. (2006). Mislocalization of perceived saccade target position induced by perisaccadic visual stimulation.Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 12-20.
  • Bartelt, R. & Darling, W. G. (2002). Opposite effects on perception and action induced by the Ponzo illusion.Experimental Brain Research, 146, 433-440.
  • Becker, W. (1972). The control of eye movements in the saccadic system. In J. Dichgans & E. Bizzi (Eds.),Cerebral control of eye movements and motion perception(pp. 233-243). Basel: Karger.
  • Bedell, H. E., & Flom, M. C. (1983). Normal and abnormal space perception.Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics, 60, 426-435.
  • Bischof, N., & Kramer, E. (1968). Investigations and considerations of directional perception during voluntary saccadic eye movements.Psychologische Forschung, 32,185-218.
  • Bocianski, D., Müsseler, J., & Erlhagen, W. (2008). Relative mislocalization of successively presented stimuli.Vision Research, 48, 2204-2212.
  • Brenner, E., Smeets, J. B. J., & van den Berg, A. V. (2001). Smooth eye movements and spatial localization.Vision Research, 41, 2253-2259.
  • Bruno, A., & Morrone, M. C. (2007). Influence of saccadic adaptation on spatial localization: Comparison of verbal and pointing reports.Journal of Vision, 7, 1-13.
  • Collins, T., Doré-Mazars, K, & Lappe, M. (2007). Motor space structures perceptual space: Evidence from human saccadic adaptation.Brain Research, 1172, 32-39.
  • Deubel, H. (1999). Separate mechanisms for the adaptive control of reactive, volitional, and memory-guided saccadic eye movements. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.),Attention and Performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application(pp. 697-721). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Deubel, H., & Hauske, G. (1988). The programming of visually guided saccades. In H. Marko, G. Hauske, & A. Struppler (Eds.),Processing structures for perception and action(pp. 119-132). Weinheim: DFGSonderforschungsberichte, VCH Verlagsgesellschaft.
  • Deubel, H., Wolf, W., & Hauske, G. (1984). The evaluation of the oculomotor error signal. In A. G. Gale & F. Johnson (Eds.),Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research(pp. 55-62). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Eggert, T., Sailer, U., Ditterich, J., & Straube, A. (2002). Differential effect of a distractor on primary saccades and perceptual localization.Vision Research, 42, 2969-2984.
  • Findlay. J. M. (1982). Global processing for saccadic eye movements.Vision Research, 22, 1033-1045.
  • Findlay, J. M., Brogan, D., & Wenban-Smith, M. G. (1993). The spatial signal for saccadic eye movements emphasizes visual boundaries.Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 633-641.
  • Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccadic generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 661-674.
  • Finney, D. J. (1971).Probit analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Georg, K. & Lappe, M. (2009). Effects of saccadic adaptation on visual localization before and during saccades.Experimental Brain Research, 192, 9-23.
  • Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002) Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 1039-1054.
  • Haffenden, A. M., Schiff, K. C., & Goodale, M. A. (2001). The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: Nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp.Current Biology, 11, 177-181.
  • Henson, D. B. (1978). Corrective saccades: Effects of altering visual feedback.Vision Research, 18, 63-67.
  • Kapoula, Z. (1985). Evidence for a range effect in the saccadic system.Vision Research, 25, 1155-1157.
  • Kerlinger, F. N., & Pedhazur, E. J. (1973).Multiple regression in behavioral research. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
  • Kerzel, D. (2002). Memory for the position of stationary objects: Disentangling foveal bias and memory averaging.Vision Research, 42, 159-167.
  • Kingstone, A., & Klein, R. M. (1993). What are human express saccades?Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 260-273.
  • Koenderink, J. J. (1990). The brain a geometry engine.Psychological Research, 52, 122-127.
  • Kowler, E., & Blaser, E. (1995). The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets.Vision Research, 35, 1741-1754.
  • Leibowitz, H. W., Myers, N. A., & Grant, D. A. (1955). Frequency of seeing and radial localization of single and multiple visual stimuli.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 369-373.
  • Lemij, H. G., & Collewijn, H. (1989). Differences in accuracy of human saccades between stationary and jumping targets.Vision Research, 29, 1737-1748.
  • Lieberman, H. R. (1983). Computation of psychophysical tresholds using the probit technique.Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 15, 446-448.
  • Mateeff, S., & Gourevich, A. (1983). Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction.Biological Cybernetics, 10, 111-118.
  • Mateeff, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (1988). Perceptual latencies are shorter for motion towards the fovea than for motion away.Vision Research, 28, 711-719.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995).The visual brain in action. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Müsseler, J., Stork, S., & Kerzel, D. (2008). Localising the onset of moving stimuli with a pointing task and a relative judgment task.Vision Research, 48, 611-617.
  • Müsseler, J., & van der Heijden, A. H. C. (2004). Two spatial maps contributing to perceived space. Evidence from a relative mislocalization.Visual Cognition, 11, 235-254.
  • Müsseler, J., van der Heijden, A. H. C., Mahmud, S. H., Deubel, H., & Ertsey, S. (1999). Relative mislocalization of briefly presented stimuli in the retinal periphery.Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1646-1661.
  • O'Regan, J. K. (1984). Retinal versus extraretinal influences in flash localization during saccadic eye movements in the presence of a visible background.Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 1-14.
  • Osaka, N. (1977). Effect of refraction on perceived locus of a target in the peripheral visual field.The Journal of Psychology, 95, 59-62.
  • Rose, D., & Halpern, D. L. (1992). Stimulus mislocalization depends on spatial frequency.Perception, 21, 289-296.
  • Rossetti, Y., & Pisella, L. (2002). Several ‘vision for action’ systems: A guide to dissociating and integrating dorsal and ventral functions. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.),Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action(pp. 62-119). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Rotman, G., Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2005). Flashes are localized as if they were moving with the eyes.Vision Research, 45, 355-364.
  • Sheliga, B. M., Riggio, L., Craighero, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Spatial attention-determined modifications in saccade trajectories.Neuroreport, 6, 585-588.
  • Skavenski, A. A. (1990). Eye movement and visual localization of objects in space. In E. Kowler (Ed.),Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes(pp. 263-287). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Tipper, S. P., Howard, L. A., & Jackson, S. R. (1997). Selective reaching to grasp: Evidence for distractor interference effects.Visual Cognition, 4, 1-38.
  • Trappenberg, T. P., Dorris, M. C., Munoz, D. P., & Klein, R. M. (2001). A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 256-271.
  • van der Heijden, A. H. C., Müsseler, J., & Bridgeman, B. (1999). On the perception of position. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.),Cognitive contribution to the perception of spatial and temporal events(pp. 19-38). North-Holland: Elsevier.
  • van der Heijden, A. H. C., van der Geest, J. N., de Leeuw, F., Krikke, K., & Müsseler, J. (1999). Sources of position-perception error for small isolated targets.Psychological Research, 62, 20-35.
  • Vos, P. G., Bocheva, N., Yamimoff, N., & Helsper, E. (1993). Perceived location of two-dimensional patterns.Vision Research, 33, 2157-2169.
  • Westheimer, G. (1981). Visual hyperacuity.Progress in Sensory Physiology, 1, 1-30.
  • Wolff, P. (1987). Perceptual learning by saccades: A cognitive approach. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Perspectives on perception and action(pp. 249-271). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0072-6
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.