Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


System messages
  • Session was invalidated!
2009 | 40 | 4 | 58-71

Article title

A Notional Level of Cognitive Distortions in Depression: Does It Exist? A Voice for Interdisciplinarity in Studying Cognitive Functioning of Individuals with Depressive Disorders

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
This aritcle raises the problem of cognitive depressive distortions observed at the notional level. It relates to recent neuropsychological, psychological, and linguistic studies, taking an interdisciplinary theoretical perspective, and illustrating the advantages of interdisciplinarity in modern psycholinguistic projects. It shows that, generally, the notional level has been neglected in psychopathological and psychological research on depressive functioning. The problem is described with reference to linguistic and psycholinguistic theories linking language with cognition. Particular emphasis is devoted to theories and methods of metaphorical processing analysis which, taking into account the similarities between cognitive functions engaged in understanding metaphors, and those disordered as a result of depression, seem to be an adequate frame to study the problem. The text ends with a proposal of an interdisciplinary research project dedicated to the study of metaphorical conceptualizations of some notions created by people suffering from depression.

Year

Volume

40

Issue

4

Pages

58-71

Physical description

Document type

ARTICLE

Contributors

  • Marlena Bartczak, Uniwersytet Warszawski, Wydzial Psychologii, ul. Stawki 5/7, 00-183 Warszawa, Poland

References

  • Addis, D. R., & McAndrews, M. P. (2006). Prefrontal and hippocampal contributions to the generation and binding of semantic associations during successful encoding.NeuroImage, 33, 1194-1206.
  • Andreasen N. C. (2003).Fascynujący mózg. Walka z chorobami psychicznymi w epoce genomu[Fascinating brain. Struggle with mental disorders in the era of the genome]. Lublin: Wydawnictwo CZELEJ.
  • Atchley, R. A., Stringer, R., Mathias, E., Ilardi, S. S., & Minatrea, A. D. (2007). The right hemisphere's contribution to emotional word processing in currently depressed, remitted depressed, and never-depressed individuals.Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 145-160.
  • Barnett, K. J., Finucane, C., Asher, J. E., Bargary, G., Corvin, A. P., Newell, F. N., & Mitchell, K. J. (2008). Familial patterns and the origins of individual differences in synaesthesia.Cognition, 106, 871-893.
  • Bartczak, M. (2008). Metaphorical conceptualization of some notions in depressive disorders: Is PLEASURE an insipid milky jelly?Psychology of Language and Communication, 12 (1), 85-102.
  • Bartczak, M. (2009).A new tool investigating metaphorical conceptualization of some notions: May it differentiate people in the area of mood?Poster presented at the European Society for Cognitive Psychology Conference, September 2009, Kraków.
  • Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions.Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324-333.
  • Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects, New York: Guilford.
  • Beck, A. T. (1973).The diagnosis and management of depression. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
  • Beck, A. T. (1976).Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.
  • Beck, A. T. (1995).Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford.
  • Beeman, M. (1998). Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension. In M. Beeman & C. Chiarello (Eds.),Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience(pp. 255-284). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Blasko, D. G. (1999). Only the tip of the iceberg: Who understands what about metaphor?Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1675-1683.
  • Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors.Cognition, 75, 1-28.
  • Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time.Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1-22.
  • Butler, C. S. (2008). Cognitive adequacy in structural-functional theories of language.Language Sciences, 30, 1-30.
  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time.Cognition, 106, 579-593.
  • Castaneda, A. E., Suvisaari, J., Marttunen, M., Perälä, J., Saarni, S. I., Aalto-Setälä, T., Aro, H., Koskinen, S., Lönnqvist, J., & Tuulio-Henriksson, A. (2008). Cognitive functioning in a population-based sample of young adults with a history of non-psychotic unipolar depressive disorders without psychiatric comorbidity.Journal of Affective Disorders, 110 (1-2), 36-45.
  • Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: Some interactions.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 55-61.
  • Chemero, A. P. (2001). Making space for embodiment [review of the bookRepresentation and behavior].Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 317-318.
  • Chiao, J. Y., Bordeaux, A. R., & Ambady, N. (2004). Mental representations of social status.Cognitive Psychology, 22, 184-210.
  • Chiappe, D. L. & Chiappe, P. (2007). The role of working memory in metaphor production and comprehension.Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 172-188.
  • Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 371-374.
  • Crawford, L. E., Regier, T., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Linguistic and nonlinguistic spatial categorization.Cognition, 75, 209-235.
  • Eichstedt, J. A., Serbin, L. A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1998). Metaphorical gender knowledge in the second year of life.Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 394.
  • Eichstedt, J. A., Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Sen, M. G. (2002). Of bears and men: Infants' knowledge of conventional and metaphorical gender stereotypes.Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 296-310.
  • Feldman Barrett, L., Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2007). Cognitive-emotional interactions: Language as context for the perception of emotion.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 327-331.
  • Feldman, G. (2006).From molecules to metaphor: A neural theory of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Feldman, J., & Narayanan, S. (2004). Embodied meaning in a neural theory of language.Brain and Language, 89, 385-392.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1975).The language of thought. New York: Crowell.
  • Fossati, P., Amar, G., Raoux, N., Ergis, A. M., & Allilaire, J. F. (1999). Executive functioning and verbal memory in young patients with unipolar depression and schizophrenia.Psychiatry Researches, 89, 171-187.
  • Foster, P. S., Drago, V., Fitzgerald, D. B., Skoblar, B. M., Crucian, G. P., & Heilman, K. M. (2008). Spreading activation of lexical-semantic networks in Parkinson's disease.Neuropsychologia, 46, 1908-1914.
  • Gallistel, C. R. (2002) Language and spatial frames of reference in mind and brain.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 321-322.
  • Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition.Cognition, 83, 49-79.
  • Gentner, D., & Bowdle, B. F. (2001). Convention, form, and figurative language processing.Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 223-247.
  • Gentner, D., Bowdle, B. Wolff, P., & Boronat, C. (2001). Metaphor is like analogy. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. Kokinov (Eds.),The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science(pp. 199-253). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Georgieff, N., Dominey, P. F., Michel, F., Marie-Cardine, M., & Dalery, J. (1998). Semantic priming in major depressive state.Psychiatry Research, 78, 29-44.
  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Keysar, B., Robertson, R. R. W., & Werner, N. K. (2001). The role of suppression and enhancement in understanding metaphors.Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 433-450.
  • Gibbs, R. (2006). Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation.Mind & Language, 21, 434-458.
  • Giora, R. (1997). Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis.Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 183-206.
  • Giora, R. (2002). Literal vs. figurative language: Different or equal?Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 487-506.
  • Giora, R., Zaidel, E., Soroker, N., Batori, G., & Kasher, A. (2000). Differential effects of right- and left-hemisphere damage on understanding sarcasm and metaphor.Metaphor and Symbol, 1, 63-83.
  • Glucksberg, S. (2001)Understanding figurative language: From metaphors to idioms. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Glucksberg, S. (2003). The psycholinguistics of metaphor.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 92-96.
  • Gollan, T. H., Salmon, D. P., & Paxton, J. L. (2006). Word association in early Alzheimer's disease.Brain and Language, 99, 289-303.
  • Gotlib, I. H., & Neubauer, D. L. (2000). Information-processing approaches to the study of cognitive biases in depression. In S. L. Johnson, A. M. Hayes, T. M. Field, N. Schneiderman, & P. M. McCabe (Eds.),Stress, coping, and depression(pp. 117-143). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.)Speech acts: Syntax and Semantics(pp. 41-58). New York: New York Academic Press.
  • Grossenbacher, P. G., & Lovelace, C. T. (2001). Mechanisms of synesthesia: Cognitive and physiological constraints.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 36-41.
  • Hayek, F. A. (1952).The sensory order. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  • Hebb, D. O. (1949).The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.
  • Holmes, E. A. & Mathews, A. (2005). Mental imagery and emotion: A special relationship?Emotion, 5, 489-497.
  • Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Dalgleish, T., & Mackintosh, B. (2006). Positive interpretation training: Effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood.Behavior Therapy, 37, 237-247.
  • Johnson, C. (1999). Metaphor vs. conflation in the acquisition of polysemy: The case of see. In M. Hiraga, C. Sinha, & S. Wilcox (Eds.),Cultural, psychological and typological issues in cognitive linguistics(pp. 155-169). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
  • Johnson, M. (1992). Philosophical implications of cognitive semantics.Cognitive Linguistics, 3-4, 28-45.
  • Jones, L. L. & Estes, Z. (2005). Metaphor comprehension as attributive categorization.Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 110-124.
  • Joorman, J., Krejtz, I., & Sędek, G. (2006). Deficyty poznawcze w depresji. Rola procesów hamowania. In M. Fajkowska, M. Marszał-Wiśniewska, & G. Sędek (Eds.),Podpatrywanie myśli i uczuć. Zaburzenia i optymalizacja procesów emocjonalnych i poznawczych. Nowe kierunki badań(pp. 148-167). Gdańsk: GWP.
  • Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 512-518.
  • Just, M. & Carpenter, P. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory.Psychological Review, 99, 122-149.
  • Kay, P. & McDaniel, C. (1978). The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms.Language, 54 (3), 610-646.
  • Keysar, B., Shen, Y., Glucksberg, S., & Horton, W. S. (2000). Conventional language: How metaphorical is it?Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 576-593.
  • Kintsch, W. (2000). Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 257-266.
  • Kintsch, W. (2001). Predication.Cognitive Science, 25, 173-202.
  • Kravchenko, A. (2006). Cognitive linguistics, biology of cognition, and biosemiotics: Bridging the gaps.Language Sciences, 28, 51-75.
  • Lakoff, G. (1987).Women, fire, and dangerous things. What categories tell us about the nature of thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.),Metaphor and thought(pp. 202-251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980a).Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980b). The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system.Cognitive Science, 4, 195-208.
  • Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1999).Philosophy in the flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
  • Lamberton, A. & Oei, T. P. S. (2008). A test of the cognitive content specificity hypothesis in depression and anxiety.Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 23-31.
  • Levinson, S. C., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Rasch, B. H. (2002). Returning the tables: Language affects spatial reasoning.Cognition, 84, 155-188.
  • Li, P. W. & Gleitman, L. R. (2002) Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning.Cognition, 83, 265-294.
  • Libura, A. (2000).Wyobraźnia w języku: Leksykalne korelaty schematów wyobrażeniowych: Centrum-Peryferie i Siły[Imagination in language: Lexical correlates of image schemes: Center-Peripheries and Force]. Wrocław: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Wrocławskiego.
  • Mahurin, R. K., Velligan, D. I., Hazleton, B., Davis, J. M., Eckert, S., & Miller, A. L. (2006). Trail making test errors and executive function in schizophrenia and depression.Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 271-288.
  • Markošová, M. (2008). Network model of human language.Physica A, 387, 661-666.
  • Mashal, N. & Faust, M. (2008). Right hemisphere sensitivity to novel metaphoric relations: Application of the signal detection theory.Brain and Language, 104, 103-112.
  • Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2007). An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions.Brain and Language, 100, 115-126.
  • McNally, R. J. (2006). Cognitive abnormalities in post-traumatic stress disorder.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 271-277.
  • Monetta, L. & Pell, M. D. (2007). Effects of verbal working memory deficits on metaphor comprehension in patients with Parkinson's disease.Brain and Language, 101, 80-89.
  • Mulvenna, C. M. & Walsh, V. (2006). Synaesthesia: Supernormal integration?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 350-352.
  • Munnich, E., Landau, B., & Dosher, B. A. (2001). Spatial language and spatial representation: A cross-linguistic comparison.Cognition, 81, 171-207.
  • Murphy, G. L. (1996). On metaphoric representation.Cognition, 60, 173-204.
  • Pinker, S. (1994).The language instinct. New York: Morrow.
  • Pużyński, S. (2002).Depresje i zaburzenia afektywne[Depressions and affective disorders]. Warszawa: PZWL.
  • Pużyński, S. & Wciórka J. (Ed.) (1997).Klasyfikacja zaburzeń psychicznych i zaburzeń zachowania w ICD-10. Opisy kliniczne i wskazówki diagnostyczne[Classification of mental and behavioral disorders in ICD-10. Clinical descriptions and diagnostic outlines]. Kraków: Vesalius.
  • Raes, F., Wiliams, J. M. G., & Hermans, D. (2009). Reducing cognitive vulnerability to depression: A preliminary investigation of MEmory Specificity Training (MEST) in inpatients with depressive symptomatology.Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40 (1), 24-38.
  • Rapp, A. M., Leube, D. T., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Kircher, T. T. (2004). Neural correlates of metaphor processing.Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 395-402.
  • Rapp, A. M., Leube, D. T., Erb, M., Grodd, W., & Kircher, T. T. J. (2007). Laterality in metaphor processing: Lack of evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging for the right hemisphere theory.Brain and Language, 100, 142-149.
  • Rosenhan, D. L., Seligman, M. E. P., & Walker E. (2003).Psychopatologia[Psychopathology]. Poznań: Zysk i S-ka.
  • Ross, T. P., Calhoun, E., Cox, T., Wenner, C., Kono, W., & Pleasant, M. (2007). The reliability and validity of qualitative scores for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 475-488.
  • Schlesinger, M. (2001). Building a better baby: Embodied models of infant cognition.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 139.
  • Schmidt, G. L., DeBuse, C. J., & Seger, C. A. (2007). Right hemisphere metaphor processing? Characterizing the lateralization of semantic processes.Brain and Language, 100, 127-141.
  • Schnitzer, M. L., & Pedreira, M. A. (2005). A neuropsychological theory of metaphor.Language Sciences, 27, 31-49.
  • Searle, J. (1979). Metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.),Metaphor and thought(pp. 91-123). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Seitz, J. A. (2000). The bodily basis of thought.New Ideas in Psychology, 18, 23-40.
  • Seitz, J. A. (2005). The neural, evolutionary, developmental, and bodily basis of metaphor.New Ideas in Psychology, 23, 74-95.
  • Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Garcia, K., Gersing, K., Pieper, C., Welsh-Bohmer, K., Steffens, D. C, & Doraiswamy, P. M. (2006). Cognitive function in late life depression: Relationships to depression severity, cerebrovascular risk factors and processing speed.Biological Psychiatry, 60, 58-65.
  • Shibata, M., Abe, J., Terao, A., & Miyamoto, T. (2007). Neural mechanisms involved in the comprehension of metaphoric and literal sentences: An fMRI study.Brain Research, 1166, 92-102.
  • Smith, D. J., Muir, W. J., & Blackwood, D. H. R. (2006). Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic young adults with bipolar spectrum disorder and recurrent major depressive disorder.Bipolar Disorder, 8, 40-46.
  • Solomon, A. & Haaga, D. A. F. (2005). Teoria i terapia poznawcza depresji [Cognitive theory and therapy of depression]. In M. A. Reinecke & D. A. Clark (Eds.),Psychoterapia poznawcza[Cognitive psychotherapy] (pp. 25-53). Gdańsk: GWP.
  • Stępnik, K. (1988).Filozofia metafory[Philosophy of metaphor]. Lublin: Wydawnictwo Lubelskie.
  • Sternberg, D. E. & Jarvik, M. E. (1976). Memory functions in depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 33 (2), 219-224.
  • Stringaris, A. K., Medford, M., Giora, R., Giampietro, V. C., Brammer, M. J., & Davida, A. S. (2006). How metaphors influence semantic relatedness judgments: The role of the right frontal cortex.Neuro-Image, 33, 784-793.
  • Tendahl, M. & Gibbs, R. W. Jr. (2008). Complementary perspectives on metaphor: Cognitive linguistics and relevance theory.Journal of Pragmatics, 40 (11), 1823-1864.
  • Thibodeau, P. & Durgin, F. H. (2008). Productive figurative communication: Conventional metaphors facilitate the comprehension of related novel metaphors.Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 521-540.
  • Thompson, E. & Varela, F. J. (2001). Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 418-425.
  • Turner, M. (1993). An image-schematic constraint on metaphor. In R. A. Geiger, & B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Eds.),Conceptualization and mental processing in language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Van der Gucht, F., Willems, K., & De Cuypere, L. (2007). The iconicity of embodied meaning. Polysemy of spatial prepositions in the cognitive framework.Language Sciences, 29, 733-754.
  • Vigliocco, G. & Kleiner, L. F. (2004). From mind in the mouth to language in the mind [Review of the bookLanguage in Mind].Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 5-7.
  • von Hecker, U. & Sędek, G. (1999). Uncontrollability, depression, and the construction of mental models: A meta-analysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 833-850.
  • Walter, H., Kiefer, M., & Erk, S. (2003). Content, context and cognitive style in mood-memory interactions. Response to Lewis and Critchley.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 433-434.
  • Ward, J. & Simner, J. (2003). Lexical-gustatory synaesthesia: Linguistic and conceptual factors.Cognition, 89, 237-261.
  • Whorf, B. L. (1956).Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Ed. by J. B. Carroll. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
  • Wilson, D. & Sperber, D. (2004). Truthfulness and relevance.Mind, 111, 583-632.
  • Xu, F. (2007). Sortal concepts, object individuation, and language.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 400-406.
  • Yu, N. (2004). The eyes for sight and mind.Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 663-686.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

CEJSH db identifier
10PLAAAA07549

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.deec776f-f807-329f-b2da-768cd795c3dd
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.