Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2011 | 42 | 2 | 56-64

Article title

Conscious access to fear-relevant information is mediated by threshold

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
The present report proposed a model of access consciousness to fear-relevant information according to which there is a threshold for emotional perception beyond that the subject makes hits with no false alarm. The model was examined by having the participants performed a confidence-ratings masking task with fearful faces. Measures of the thresholds for conscious access were taken by looking at the receiver operating characteristics (ROC) curves generated from a three-state low- and high-threshold (3-LHT) model by Krantz. Indeed, the analysis of the masking data revealed that the ROCs had threshold-like-nature (a two-limb shape) rather continuous (a curvilinear shape) challenging in this fashion the classical signal-detection view on perceptual processing. Moreover, the threshold ROC curve exhibited the specific y-intercepts relevant to conscious access performance. The study suggests that the threshold can be an intrinsic property of conscious access, mediating emotional contents between perceptual states and consciousness.

Keywords

Year

Volume

42

Issue

2

Pages

56-64

Physical description

Contributors

  • Wroclaw Faculty of Psychology, Warsaw School of Social Sciences and Humanities, 30 Ostrowskiego Street, 53-238 Wroclaw, Poland

References

  • Baars, B. J. (1993). Why volition is a foundation issue for psychology.Conscious. Cogn, 2, 281-309.
  • Baars, B. J. (1998). Metaphors of consciousness and attention in the brain.Trends in Neurosciences, 21(2), 58-62.
  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227-227
  • Block, N. (2008). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5-6), 481-499.
  • Chalmers, D. J. (2000). What is a neural correlate of consciousness. In Metzinger, T.Neural correlates of consciousness: Empirical and conceptual questions.The MIT Press, 17-40.
  • de Gelder, B., Pourtois, G., van Raamsdonk, M., Vroomen, J., & Weiskrantz, L. (2001). Unseen stimuli modulate conscious visual experience: evidence from interhemispheric summation.Neuroreport, 12(2), 385.
  • Dehaene, S. (2008). Conscious and nonconscious processes: distinct forms of evidence accumulation. InDecision making, the human mind, and implications for institutions.(Engel C, Singer W, eds) (p. 21-49).
  • Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204-211.
  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976).Pictures of Facial Affect.Paul Ekman.
  • Esteves, F., & Ohman, A. (1993). Masking the face: Recognition of emotional facial expressions as a function of the parameters of backward masking.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 34(1), 1-18.
  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966).Signal detection theory and psychophysics.Wiley, NewYork.
  • Johnson, M. H. (2005). Subcortical face processing.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 766-774.
  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (2004). Availability, accessibility, and subliminal perception.Consciousness and Cognition, 13(1), 92-100.
  • Kouider, S., & Dupoux, E. (2004). Partial awareness creates the" illusion" of subliminal semantic priming.Psychological Science-Cambridge, vol. 15, 75-81.
  • Krantz, D. H. (1969). Threshold theories of signal detection.Psychol Rev, 76(3), 308-24.
  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces - KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, ISBN 91-630-7164-9.
  • Luce, R. D. (1963). A threshold theory for simple detection experiments.Psychological Review, 70(1).
  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005).Detection Theory: A User's Guide.Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc Inc.
  • Merikle, P. M., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J. D. (2001). Perception without awareness: perspectives from cognitive psychology.Cognition, 79(1-2), 115-134.
  • Morris, J. S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field.Brain, 124(6), 1241.
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning.Psychological review, 108(3), 483.
  • Phillips, M. L., Williams, L. M., Heining, M., Herba, C. M., Russell, T., Andrew, C., et al. (2004). Differential neural responses to overt and covert presentations of facial expressions of fear and disgust.Neuroimage, 21(4), 1484-1496.
  • Press, W. H., Flannery, P. P., Teukolsky, S. A., & Vetterling, W. T. (1986).Numerical recipes.Cambridge Univ. Pr.
  • Slotnick, S. D., & Dodson, C. S. (2005). Support for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory.Memory & Cognition, 33(1), 151
  • Szczepanowski, R., & Pessoa, L. (2007). Fear perception: Can objective and subjective awareness measures be dissociated.Journal of Vision, 7(4), 10.
  • Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(4), 381-391.
  • Tsuchiya, N., & Adolphs, R. (2007). Emotion and consciousness.Trends in cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 158-167.
  • Vuilleumier, P., & Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging.Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 174-194.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.desklight-e94f5ad6-bf77-4a57-9e0c-2e31629e1fb9
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.