Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2016 | 47 | 1 | 112-122

Article title

Field Dependence, Efficiency of Information Processing in Working Memory and Susceptibility to Orientation Illusions among Architects

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
This study examined cognitive predictors of susceptibility to orientation illusions: Poggendorff, Ponzo, and Zöllner. It was assumed that lower efficiency of information processing in WM and higher field dependence are conducive to orientation illusions. 61 architects (30 women) aged M = 29, +/- 1.6, and 49 university students (29 women) aged M = 23.53, +/- 4.24, were tested with Witkin’s EFT to assess their field dependence; the SWATT method was used as a measure of WM efficiency, and susceptibility to visual illusions was verified with a series of computer tasks. We obtained a small range of the explained variance in the regression models including FDI and WM indicators. On the basis of WM efficiency indicators, we managed to confirm the existence of memory predictors of susceptibility to illusions (they are rather weak, as they explain from 6% to 14% of the variance of the dependent variable). Among the architects, lower efficiency of WM processing (weaker inhibition, task-switching) and higher field dependence are responsible for greater susceptibility to orientation illusions.

Year

Volume

47

Issue

1

Pages

112-122

Physical description

Dates

published
2016-04-01
online
2016-05-14

Contributors

  • Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
  • Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland

References

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1999). Essentials of Human Memory. UK: Psychology Press Ltd.
  • Barrett, G. V., Thornton, C. L. (1967). Cognitive style differences between engineers and college students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 25, 789-793.[Crossref]
  • Bednarek, H. (2011). Czy piloci ulegają złudzeniom percepcyjnym? Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne GWP.
  • Changizi, M. A., Widders, D. M. (2002). Latency correction explains the classical geometrical illusions. Perception 31(10), 1241-1262.[Crossref]
  • Chuderski, A., Nęcka, E. (2004). Sprawność uwagi a inteligencja: model parametryczny w architekturze poznawczej ACT-R. Studia Psychologiczne, 42, 1, 111-124.
  • Coren, S., Girgus, J., Erlichman, H., Hakstian, A. (1976). An empirical taxonomy of visual illusions. Perception and Psychophysics, 20(2), 129-137.[Crossref]
  • Coren, S., Porac, C. (1987). Individual differences in visual-geometric illusions: Predictions from measures of spatial cognitive abilities. Perception and Psychophysics, 41(3), 211-219.[Crossref]
  • Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. New York-Oxford: Oxford University Press-Clarendon Press.
  • Daini, R., Wenderoth, P. (2008). Orientation illusions vary in size and direction as a function of task-dependant attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(7), 1289-1297.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Dassonville, P., Walter, E., Lunger, K. A. (2006). Illusions of space, field dependence and the efficiency of working memory. Journal of Vision, 6, 6. doi: 10.1167/6.6.476.[Crossref]
  • Day, R. H. (2010). On the common stimulus condition and explanation of the Müller-Lyer, Poggendorff and Zöllner illusions: The basis for a class of geometrical illusions. Australian Journal of Psychology, 62(3), 115-120.[WoS][Crossref]
  • De Fockert, J., Wu, S. (2009). High working memory load leads to more Ebbinghaus illusion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(7), 961-970.[Crossref]
  • Ekstrom, R. B., & French, J. W. (1976). The Hidden Patterns Test. In H. Harman & D. Derman (Eds.), Educational Testing Service. Princeton, NJ.
  • Feldman-Barrett, L., Tugade, M. M., i Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 553-573.
  • Gregory, R. L. (1997). Knowledge in perception and illusion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 352, 1121-1128.[WoS]
  • Gregory, R. L. (2005). The Medawar Lecture 2001. Knowledge for vision: vision for knowledge. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 360 (1458), 1231-1251.
  • Huteau, M. (1983). Zależność-niezależność od pola i rozwój myślenia operacyjnego. Przegląd Psychologiczny, 2, 253-286.
  • Jahoda, G., Stacey, B. (1970). Susceptibility to geometrical illusions according to culture and professional training. Perception and Psychophysics, 7(3), 179-184.[Crossref]
  • Kholodnaya, M. A. (2002). Kognitiivnii stili: O prirode individual’nogouma [Cognitive styles: On the nature of individual mind]. Moscow: PER SE-Jossey-Bass.
  • Króliczak, G. (1999). Błędy percepcyjne czy percepcje normalne? Mechanizmy powstawania iluzji percepcyjnych. W: A. Klawiter, L. Nowak, P. Przybysz (red.) Umysł a rzeczywistość. Poznańskie studia z fi lozofi i humanistyki, t.5(18), 303-325.
  • Lester, B. D, Dassonville, P. (2011). Attentional control settings modulate susceptibility to the induced Roeloffs effect. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1398-1406.[WoS]
  • Maheux, M. J., Townsend, J. C., Gresock C. J. (1960). Geometrical factors in illusions of direction. The American Journal of Psychology, 73(4), 535-543.[Crossref]
  • McKenna, F. P. (1984). Measures of field dependence: Cognitive style or cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 593-603.[Crossref]
  • Meneely, J., Portillo, M. (2005). The adaptable mind in design: Relating personality, cognitive style, and creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 2&3, 155-166.[Crossref]
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., Howerter, A., Wager, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contribution to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.[Crossref]
  • Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, N. C., Gilchrist J. D. (2002). Working Memory and the Suppression of Reflexive Saccades, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 95-103.[Crossref]
  • Miyake, A., Witzki, A., Emerson, M. (2001). Field dependence-independence from a working memory perspective: A dual-task investigation of the Hidden Figures Test. Memory, 9(4/5/6), 445-457.[Crossref]
  • Morris, T. L., Bergum, B. O. (1978). A note on the relationship between field- independence and creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 1114.[Crossref]
  • Navalpakkam, V., Itti, L. (2003). Sharing Resources: Buy Attention, Get Recognition, In: Proc. International Workshop on Attention and Performance in Computer Vision (WAPCV’03), Graz, Austria.
  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.[Crossref]
  • Ninio, J. (2014). Geometrical illusions are not always where you think they are: a review of some classical and less classical illusions, and ways to describe them. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 856. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00856.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Orzechowski, J., Bednarek, H. (2004). Uwaga jako mechanizm zróżnicowania struktur poznawczych. Studia Psychologiczne, 42(1), 125-137.
  • Prinzmetal, W., Beck, D. (2001). The tilt-constancy theory of visual illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(1), 206-217.[Crossref]
  • Repovs, G., Baddeley, A. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience, 139, 5-21.
  • Rock, I., Linnett, C. M., Grant, P. (1992). Perception without attention: Results of a New Method. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 502-534.[Crossref]
  • Schachar, R., Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26, 710-720.[Crossref]
  • Shulman, G. L. (1992). Attentional modulation of size contrast. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 45(4), 529-546.[Crossref]
  • Smeets, J. B., Brenner, E. (2006). 10 Years of Illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1501-1504.[Crossref]
  • Strzałecki, A. (1973). Czynniki powodzenia w studiach przygotowujących do zawodu projektanta. Warszawa: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.
  • Tsal, Y. (1984). A Muller-Lyer illusion induced by selective attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 36(2), 319-333. doi:10.1080/14640748408402162.[Crossref]
  • Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438, 500-503.
  • Walter, E., Dassonville, P. (2007). In search of the hidden: contextual processing in parietal cortex. Journal of Vision, 7(9). doi: 10.1167/7.9.1061.[Crossref]
  • Walter, E., Dassonville, P. (2008). Visuospatial contextual processing in the parietal cortex: an fMRI investigation of the induced Roelofs effect. Neuroimage, 42, 1686-1697.[Crossref][WoS]
  • Walter, E., Dassonville, P. (2011). Activation in a Frontoparietal Cortical Network Underlies Individual Differences in the Performance of an Embedded Figures Task. PLoS ONE 6(7): e20742. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0020742.[Crossref]
  • Witkin, H. A., Asch, S. E. (1948a). Studies in space orientation: III. Perception of the upright in the absence of a visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(5), 603-614.[Crossref]
  • Witkin, H. A., Asch, S. E. (1948b). Studies in space orientation: IV. Further experiments on perception of the upright with displaced visual fields. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(6), 762-782.[Crossref]
  • Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., Karp, S. A. (1971). A Manual for the Embedded Figures Test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R. (2007). Perceived slant: A dissociation between perception and action. Perception, 36, 249-257.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Wright, R. D., Ward, L. M. (2008). Orienting of attention. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_1515_ppb-2016-0012
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.