Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2014 | 45 | 3 | 268-274

Article title

Two routes to closure: Time pressure and goal activation effects on executive control

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
In the present study the impact of need for cognitive closure (NFC) manipulations via time pressure and explicit closure goal activation on executive control was investigated. Although there is some evidence that NFC, measured as an individual variable, is related to better performing in attentional tasks involving executive control, these results have never been validated across different manipulations of NFC. Thus, in the present study we induced NFC via internal and external time pressure and tested the impact of these manipulations on the performance in tasks that measure executive control, i.e., the Stroop and switching tasks. The results revealed that induced high (vs. low) NFC, indeed boosted performance in executive control tasks. Moreover, there was no difference in the effect of both NFC manipulations on task performance. The implications regarding the role of executive control and specific NFC manipulations in social cognition are discussed.

Year

Volume

45

Issue

3

Pages

268-274

Physical description

Dates

published
2014-09-01
online
2014-09-09

Contributors

  • Jagiellonian University
  • Institute of Psychology, al. Mickiewicza 3, 31-120 Kraków, Poland;
  • Jagiellonian University
  • Jagiellonian University

References

  • Altamirano, L. J., Miyake, A., Whitmer, A. J. (2010). When Mental Inflexibility Facilitates Executive Control: Beneficial Side Effects of Ruminative Tendencies on Goal Maintenance. Psychological Science, 21, 1377-1382. doi:10.1177/0956797610381505[WoS][PubMed][Crossref]
  • Amodio, D., Jost, J., Masters, S. & Yee, C. (2008). Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1246-1247.[WoS]
  • Bar-Tal, Y., Kishon-Rabin, L., & Tabak, N. (1997). The effect of need and ability to achieve cognitive structuring on cognitive structuring. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1158-1176. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.73.6.1158[Crossref]
  • Blais, C., & Bunge, S. (2010). Behavioral and Neural Evidence for Itemspecific Performance Monitoring. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2758-2767.[WoS][Crossref][PubMed]
  • Bukowski, M., Sędek, G., Kossowska, M., & Trejtowicz, M. (2012). The impact of background category information on the creation of social cliques: The role of need for cognitive closure and decisiveness. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 43, 12-19.
  • Bukowski, M., von Hecker, U., & Kossowska, M. (2013). Motivational determinants of reasoning about social relations: The role of need for cognitive closure. Thinking and Reasoning, 19, 150-177. doi:10.108 0/13546783.2012.752407[Crossref]
  • Chajut, E., & Algom, D. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: Implications for theories of social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 231-248. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.231[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Chiu, C., Morris, M. W., Hong, Y., & Melon, T. (2000). Motivated cultural cognition: The impact of implicit cultural theories on dispositional attribution varies as a function of need for closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 247-259.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Dijksterhuis, A., van Knippenberg, A., Kruglanski, A. W., & Schaper, C. (1996). Motivated social cognition: Need for closure effects on memory and judgement. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 32, 254-270.
  • Driscoll, D.M., Hamilton, D.L., & Sorrentino, R.M. (1991). Uncertainty orientation and recall of person-descriptive information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 494-500.[Crossref]
  • Ford, T. E., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1995). Effects of epistemic motivations on the use of accessible constructs in social judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 950-962.[Crossref]
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 116-124.[Crossref]
  • Freund, T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Shpitzajzen, A. (1985). The freezing and unfreezing of impressional primacy: Effects of the need for structure and the fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 479-487.[Crossref]
  • Heaton, A. W., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1991). Person perception by introverts and extroverst under time pressure: Effects of need for closure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 161-165.[Crossref]
  • Inzlicht, M., McGregor, I., Hirsh, J. & Ash, K. (2009). Neural markers of religious conviction. Psychological Science, 20, 385-392.[WoS][Crossref][PubMed]
  • Jamieson, D. W., & Zanna, M. P. (1989). Need for structure in attitude formation and expression. In A. Pratkanis, S. Breckler, & A. G. Greenwald ( Eds. ), Attitude structure and function ( pp. 46-68 ). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum.
  • Kossowska, M. (2007a). The role of cognitive inhibition in motivation toward closure. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1117-1126.[Crossref]
  • Kossowska, M. (2007b). Motivation toward closure and cognitive processes: An individual differences approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 2149-2158.[Crossref]
  • Kossowska, M., Orehek, E., Kruglanski, A.W. (2010). Motivation towards closure and cognitive resources: An individual differences approach. In A. Gruszka, G. Mathews, and B. Szymura (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition: attention, memory and executive control (pp. 369-382). New York, NY US: Springer Science + Business Media.
  • Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. New York: Psychology Press.
  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and un-freezing of lay-inferences: Effects of impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 448-468.[Crossref]
  • Kruglanski, A. W., Freund, T., & Bar-Tal, D. (1996). Motivational effects in the mere-exposure paradigm. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 479-499.[Crossref]
  • Kruglanski, A. W., Dechesne, M., Orehek, E., & Pierro, A. (2009). Three decades of lay epistemics: The why, how, and who of knowledge formation. European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 146-191. doi:10.1080/10463280902860037[WoS][Crossref]
  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” and “Freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263-283.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Kruglanski, A. W., Webster, D. M., & Klem, A. (1993). Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 861-877.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Li, C., Zhang, S., Duann, J., Yan, P., Sinha, R., & Mazure, C. (2009). Gender differences in cognitive control: An extended investigation of the stop signal task. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 3, 262-276. DOI 10.1007/s11682-009-9068-1[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Marzecová, A., Bukowski, M., Boros, M., Correa, A., Lupiáñez, J., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). Tracing the bilingual advantage in cognitive control: The role of flexibility in temporal preparation and category switching. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 586-604.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Minear, M., & Park, D. C. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 630-633. doi:10.3758/BF03206543[Crossref]
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N., Emerson, M., Witzki, A., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contribution to complex ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.[Crossref]
  • Richter, L., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1998). Seizing on the latest. Motivationally driven recency effects in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 313-329.[Crossref]
  • Roets, A., & Van Hiel, A. (2011). Impaired performance as a source of reduced energy investment in judgment under stressors. (European) Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 625-632.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Roets, A., Van Hiel, A., Cornelis, I., & Soetens, B. (2008). Determinants of task performance and invested effort: A need for closure by ability interaction analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 779-792.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop Effect: Why proportion Congruent Has Nothing to Do With Congruency and Everything to Do With Contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory & Cognition, 34(3), 514-523. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.514[WoS][Crossref]
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User’s Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies on interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662. Retrieved from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Stroop Tzelgov, J., Henik, A., & Berger, J. (1992). Controlling Stroop effects by manipulating expectations for color words. Memory & Cognition, 20, 727-735.
  • Webster, D. M. (1993a). Motivated augmentation and reduction of the overattribution bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 261-271.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Webster, D. M. (1993b). Groups under the influence: Need for closure effects on information sharing in decision making groups. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Maryland, College Park.
  • Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049-1062.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Webster, D. M., Richter, L., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). On leaping to conclusions when feeling tired: Mental fatigue effects on impressional primacy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 181-195.[Crossref]
  • West, R., & Alain, C. (2000). Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task. Brain Research, 873, 102-111.
  • Zivnuska, S., Kiewitz, C., Hochwater, W. A., Perrewe, P. L., & Zellars, K. L. (2002). What is too much or too little? The curvilinear effects of job tension on turnover intent, value attainment, and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1344-1360. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01440.x [Crossref]

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_2478_ppb-2014-0033
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.