PL EN


2011 | 49 | 5 | 77-88
Article title

Is the executive function necessary for inferring mental states of other people? evidence from studies on patients with brain impairment

Authors
Content
Title variants
Languages of publication
EN
Abstracts
EN
Theory of mind (ToM) refers to the ability to represent the mental states of others. The Executive Function (EF) refers to higher-level cognitive processes encompassing planning, inhibition, coordination, shifting, and coordination of action sequences. Current studies on ToM and EF suggest that these two cognitive abilities might be functionally or anatomically linked. Although the relation between ToM and EF has been widely investigated, the results remain inconclusive particularly when considering the functional architecture of a mature brain system. The present study aims to seek this lacuna with the use of neuropsychological methodology. The pattern of ToM and EF defi cits within patients with brain injury were investigated. We compared the performance of four patients with a set of tasks examining theory of mind abilities and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) assessing EF functioning. Results yielded a dissociation between ToM and EF, suggesting that in an adults’ brain executive functions are not necessary for inferring the mental states of others.
Year
Volume
49
Issue
5
Pages
77-88
Physical description
Dates
published
2011-11-01
online
2012-11-13
Contributors
References
  • Ahmad, A.-A. (2003). A neurobiological mapping of theory of mind. Brain Research Reviews, 43(1), 29-40.
  • Alfredo, A. (2008). On the evolutionary origins of executive functions. Brain and Cognition, 68(1), 92-99.[WoS]
  • Apperly, I. A., Samson, D. i Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 190-201.[WoS][PubMed][Crossref]
  • Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., Chiavarino, C. i Humphreys, G. W. (2004). Frontal and Temporo-Parietal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind: Neuropsychological Evidence from a False-Belief Task with Reduced Language and Executive Demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 1773-1784. [Crossref]
  • Apperly, I.A., Samson, D. i Humphreys, G.W. (2005). Domain-specifi city and theory of mind: Evaluating evidence from neuropsychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (12), 572-577.[Crossref]
  • Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W. i Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 170-177.[Crossref]
  • Bach, L. J., Happe, F., Fleminger, S. i Powell, J. (2000). Theory of mind: Independence of executive function and the role of the frontal cortex in acquired brain injury. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5(3), 175-192.[Crossref]
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., D. I Cohen, D. (2000). Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford University Press.
  • Brune, M., Brune-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind-evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 437-455.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Carlson, S.M., Moses L.J. i Claxton L. J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: an investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 87, 299-319.
  • Crawford, J.R. Howell, D.C. (1998). Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 482-486.[Crossref]
  • Crawford, J.R., Garthwaite, P.H. (2002). Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1196-1208.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Culham, J. C., Cavanagh, P., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fMRI Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load. Neuron, 32(4), 737-745. doi:10.1016/S0896- 6273(01)00499-8 [Crossref][PubMed]
  • Dennis, M., Agostino, A., Roncadin, C., & Levin, H. (2009). Theory of mind depends on domaingeneral executive functions of working memory and cognitive inhibition in children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(7), 835-847. doi:10.1080/13 803390802572419[Crossref][WoS]
  • Gallagher, H. ., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. ., Frith, U., & Frith, C. . (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11-21.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • German, T., Hehman, J. (2006). Representational and executive selection resources in ‗theory of mind‘: Evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age. Cognition, 101(1), 129-152.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Greve, K.W., Brooks, J., Crouch, J. A.,Williams, M. C. i Rice,W. J. (1997). Factorial structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 283-285.[Crossref]
  • Havet-Thomassin, V., Allain, P., Etcharry-Bouyx, F. i Le Gall, D. (2006). What about theory of mind after severe brain injury? Brain Injury, 20(1), 83-91.[Crossref]
  • Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Crawford, J. R., Ietswaart, M., & Summers, F. (2006). Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: The role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1623-1628.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Leslie, A.M., Thaiss L. (1992). Domain specifi city in conceptual development: neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition 43, 225-251.[Crossref]
  • Muller, F., Simion, A., Reviriego, E., Galera, C., Mazaux, J.-M., Barat, M. i Joseph, P.-A. (2010). Exploring theory of mind after severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 46(9), 1088-1099.[Crossref][PubMed][WoS]
  • Perner, J., Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337-344.[Crossref]
  • Roth, D., & Leslie, A M. (1998). Solving belief problems: toward a task analysis. Cognition, 66(1), 1-31.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J. i Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind. A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 74-81.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Samson, D., Apperly I., Chiavarino C. i Humphreys G. (2004). Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else‘s belief. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 499-500.[Crossref]
  • Saxe, R. (2006). Why and how to study Theory of Mind with fMRI. Brain Research , 1079(1), 57-65.
  • Saxe, R., Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage, 19, 1835-1842.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Saxe, R., Schulz, L. i Jiang, Y. (2006b). Reading Minds versus Following Rules Social Neuroscience, 1 (3-4), 284-298.
  • Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Shamay-Tsoory, Simone G., Aharon-Peretz, Judith. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3054-3067.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Tirassa, M., Bosco, F.,M. i Colle, L. (2006). Rethinking the ontogeny of mindreading. Consciousness and Cognition 15, 197-217.[Crossref]
  • Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler’s problem with false beliefs and “false” photographs. Cognition, 35(1), 41-68.[Crossref]
  • ---
Document Type
Publication order reference
Identifiers
YADDA identifier
bwmeta1.element.doi-10_2478_v10167-010-0042-8
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.