Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2007 | 3 | 1-2 | 41-55

Article title

Binding binding: Departure points for a different version of the perceptual retouch theory

Authors

Selected contents from this journal

Title variants

Languages of publication

Abstracts

EN
In the perceptual retouch theory, masking and related microgenetic phenomena were explained as a result of interaction between specific cortical representational systems and the non-specific sub-cortical modulation system. Masking appears as deprivation of sufficient modulation of the consciousness mechanism suffered by the target-specific signals because of the temporal delay of non-specific modulation (necessary for conscious representation), which explicates the later-coming mask information instead of the already decayed target information. The core of the model envisaged relative magnitudes of EPSPs of single cortical cells driven by target and mask signals at the moment when the nonspecific, presynaptic, excitatory input arrives from the thalamus. In the light of the current evidence about the importance of synchronised activity of specific and non-specific systems in generating consciousness, the retouch theory requires perhaps a different view. This article presents some premises for modification of the retouch theory, where instead of the cumulative presynaptic spike activities and EPSPs of single cells, the oscillatory activity in the gamma range of the participating systems is considered and shown to be consistent with the basic ideas of the retouch theory. In this conceptualisation, O-binding refers to specific encoding which is based on gamma-band synchronised oscillations in the activity of specific cortical sensory modules that represent features and objects; C-binding refers to the gamma-band oscillations in the activity of the non-specific thalamic systems, which is necessary for the O-binding based data to become consciously experienced.

Year

Volume

3

Issue

1-2

Pages

41-55

Physical description

Contributors

  • Department of Psychology and Institute of Law, University of Tartu

References

  • Schiff, N. D., & Purpura, K. P. (2002). Towards a neurophysiological foundation for cognitive neuromodulation through deep brain stimulation.Thalamus & Related Systems, 2, 55-69.
  • Sheer, D. E. (1984). Focused arousal, 40 Hz EEG, and dysfunction. In T. Elbert, B. Rockstroh, W. Lutzenberger, & N. Birbaumer (Eds.),Self regulation of the brain and behavior(pp. 6-84). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Doesburg, S. M., Kitajo, K., & Ward, L. M. (2005). Increased gamma-band synchrony precedes switching of conscious perceptual objects in binocular rivalry.NeuroReport, 16, 1139-1142.[PubMed]
  • Edelman, G. M., & Tononi, G. (2000).A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagination.New York: Basic Books.
  • Elliott, M.A., & Müller, H.J. (1998). Synchronous information presented in 40-Hz flicker enhances visual feature binding.Psychological Science, 9, 277-283.
  • Aschersleben, G., & Bachmann, T. (2004).Synchronisation and metacontrast stimulation: evidence for the dual-process attentional theory.(unpublished manuscript).
  • Baars, B. J. (1988).A cognitive theory of consciousness.Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Baars, B. J. (1997).In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Bachmann, T. (1984). The process of perceptual retouch: nonspecific afferent activation dynamics in explaining visual masking.Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 69-84.
  • Bachmann, T. (1989). Microgenesis as traced by the transient paired-forms paradigm.Acta Psychologica, 70, 3-17.[PubMed]
  • Bachmann, T. (1994).Psychophysiology of visual Masking: The fine structure of conscious experience.Commack, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
  • Bachmann, T. (1999). Twelve spatiotemporal phenomena, and one explanation. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.),Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events(pp. 173-206). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Bachmann, T. (2006). Microgenesis of perception: conceptual, psychophysical, and neurobiological aspects. In H. Öğmen, & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.),The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes(pp. 11-33). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bachmann, T., & Allik, J. (1976). Integration and interruption in the masking of form by form.Perception, 5, 79-97.[PubMed]
  • Bachmann, T., Põder, E., & Luiga, I. (2004). Illusory reversal of temporal order: the bias to report a dimmer stimulus as the first.Vision Research, 44, 241-246.[PubMed]
  • Bachmann, T., & Sikka, P. (2005). Perception of successive targets presented in invariant-item streams.Acta Psychologica, 120, 19-34.[PubMed]
  • Becker, M. W., & Anstis, S. (2004). Metacontrast masking is specific to luminance polarity.Vision Research, 44, 2537-2543.[PubMed]
  • Bogen, J. E. (1995). On the neurophysiology of consciousness: I. An overview.Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 52-62.[PubMed]
  • Börgers, C., Epstein, S., & Kopell, N. J. (2005). Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: A computational study.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102, 7002-7007.[PubMed]
  • Breitmeyer, B. G. (1984):Visual masking: An integrative approach.Oxford: Clarendon.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., & Öğmen, H. (2000). Recent models and findings in backward masking: A comparison, review and update.Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1572-1595.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., & Öğmen, H. (2006).Visual masking: time slices through conscious and unconscious vision.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Öğmen, H., Ramon, J., & Chen, J. (2005). Unconscious and conscious priming by forms and their parts.Visual Cognition, 12, 720-736.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Kafaligönül, H., Öğmen, H., Mardon, L., Todd, S., & Ziegler, R. (2006). Meta- and paracontrast reveal differences between contour- and brightness-processing mechanisms.Vision Research, 46, 2654-2658.[PubMed]
  • Bremer, F. (1935). Cerveau "isolé" et physiologie du sommeil.Comptes Rendus Sociěté de Biologie, 118, 1235-1241.
  • Brooks, B., & Jung, R. (1973). Neuronal physiology of the visual cortex. In R. Jung (Ed.),Handbook of sensory physiology. Vol. VII/3: Central processing of visual information. Part B.(pp. 325-440). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Busch, N. A., Schadow, J., Fründ, I., & Herrmann, C. S. (2006). Time-frequency analysis of target detection reveals an early interface between bottom-up and top-down processes in the gamma-band.NeuroImage, 29, 1106-1116.[PubMed]
  • Churchland, P. S., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1992).The computational brain.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Cleeremans, A. (Ed.) (2003).The unity of consciousness: binding, integration, and dissociation.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Cohene, L. S., & Bechtoldt, H. P. (1974). Visual recognition as a function of stimulus offset asynchrony and duration.Perception & Psychophysics, 15, 221-226.
  • Crick, F. (1984). Function of the thalamic reticular complex: The searchlight hypothesis.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 81, 4586-4590.[PubMed]
  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness.Nature Neuroscience, 6, 119-126.[PubMed]
  • de Gelder, B., de Haan, E., & Heywood, C. (Eds.) (2001).Out of mind: Varieties of unconscious processes.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework.Cognition, 79, 1-37.[PubMed]
  • Engel, A. K., Fries, P., & Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 704-716.[PubMed]
  • Engel, A. K., & Singer, W. (2001). Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 16-25.[PubMed]
  • Enns, J. T. (2004). Object substitution and its relation to other forms of visual masking.Vision Research, 44, 1321-1331.[PubMed]
  • Enns, J. T., Lleras, A., & Di Lollo, V. (2006). A reentrant view of visual masking, object substitution, and response priming. In H. Öğmen, & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.),The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes(pp. 127-147). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Eriksen, C. W., & Collins, J. F. (1969). Visual perceptual rate under two conditions of search.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 489-492.[PubMed]
  • Fries, P., Neuenschwander, S., Engel, A. K., Goebel, R., & Singer, W. (2001). Rapid feature selective neuronal synchronization through correlated latency shifting.Nature Neuroscience, 4, 194-200.[PubMed]
  • Fröhlich, F. W. (1923). Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit.Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie, 54, 58-78.
  • Hassler, R. (1978). Interaction of reticular activating system for vigilance and the truncothalamic and pallidal systems for directing awareness and attention under striatal control. In P. A. Buser and A. Rougeul-Buser (Eds.),Cerebral correlates of conscious experience.(pp. 111-129). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
  • Haynes, J.-D., Driver, J., & Rees, G. (2005). Visibility reflects dynamic changes of effective connectivity between V1 and fusiform cortex.Neuron, 46, 811-821.[PubMed]
  • He, S., Cavanagh, P., & Intriligator, J. (1996). Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness.Nature, 383, 334-337.[PubMed]
  • Herrmann, C. S., & Mecklinger, A. (2001). Gamma activity in human EEG is related to high-speed memory comparisons during object selective attention.Visual Cognition, 8, 593-608.
  • Herzog, M. H. (2006). The relationship of visual masking and basic object recognition in healthy observers and patients with schizophrenia. In H. Öğmen, & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.),The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes(pp. 259-274). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Ishikawa, A., Shimegi, S., & Sato, H. (2006). Metacontrast masking suggests interaction between visual pathways with different spatial and temporal properties.Vision Research, 46, 2130-2138.[PubMed]
  • Jaśkowski, P., van der Lubbe, R., Schlotterbeck, E., & Verleger, R. (2002). Traces left on visual selective attention by stimuli that are not consciously identified.Psychological Science, 13, 48-54.[PubMed]
  • Jung, R. (1958). Coordination of specific and nonspecific afferent impulses at single neurons of the visual cortex. In H.H. Jasper et al. (Eds.),Reticular formation of the brain.(pp. 423-434). Boston: Little & Brown.
  • Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D.R. Davies (Eds.),Varieties of attention.(pp. 29-61). New York: Academic Press.
  • Kanwisher, N. (2001). Neural events and perceptual awareness.Cognition, 79, 89-113.[PubMed]
  • Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (2004). Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight.Neuropsychologia, 42, 831-835.[PubMed]
  • Kinoshita, S., & Lupker, S. J. (Eds.) (2003).Masked priming: The sate of the art.New York: Psychology Press.
  • Koch, C. (2004).The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach.Englewood: Roberts.
  • Kotchoubey, B. (2005). Apallic syndrome is not apallic: Is vegetative state vegetative?Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15, 333-356.[PubMed]
  • Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12-18.[PubMed]
  • Llinás, R. R. (2001).I of the vortex: From neurons to self.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Llinás, R., & Ribary, U. (2001). Consciousnes and the brain. The thalamocortical dialogue in health and disease.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 929, 166-175.[PubMed]
  • Llinás, R., Leznik, E., & Urbano, F. J. (2002). Temporal binding via cortical coincidence detection of specific and nonspecific thalamocortical inputs: A voltage-dependent dye-imaging study in mouse brain slices.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99, 449-454.[PubMed]
  • Llinás, R., Ribary, U., Contreras, D., & Pedroarena, C. (1998): The neuronal basis for consciousness.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B, 353, 1841-1849.[PubMed]
  • Llinás, R., Urbano, F. J., Leznik, E., Ramirez, R. R., & van Merle, H. J. F. (2005): Rhythmic and dysrhythmic thalamocortical dynamics: GABA systems and the edge effect.Trends in Neurosciences, 28, 325-333.[PubMed]
  • Luiga, I., & Bachmann, T. (in press). Different effects of the two types of spatial pre-cueing: what precisely is "attention" in Di Lollo's and Enns' substitution masking theory?Psychological Research.[PubMed]
  • Macknick, S. L., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2004). Dichoptic visual masking reveals that early binocular neurons exhibit weak interocular suppression: Implications for binocular vision and visual awareness.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1049-1059.[PubMed]
  • Magoun, H. W. (1958):The waking brain.Springfield, Illinois: C. C. Thomas.
  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes.Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139-170.[PubMed]
  • Melcher, D., Papathomas, T. V., & Vidnyanszky, Z. (2005). Implicit attentional selection of bound visual features.Neuron, 46, 723-729.[PubMed]
  • Melcher, D., & Vidnyanszky, Z. (2006). Subthreshold features of visual objects: Unseen but not unbound.Vision Research, 46, 1863-1867.[PubMed]
  • Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments on visual masking and word recognition.Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197-237.[PubMed]
  • Michaels, C. F., & Turvey, M. T. (1979). Central sources of visual masking: Indexing structures supporting seeing at a single, brief glance.Psychological Research, 41, 1-61.[PubMed]
  • Morris, J. S., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala.Nature, 393, 467-470.[PubMed]
  • Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H.W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the eöectroencephalogram.Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 455-473.
  • Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (2002). The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99, 9527-9532.[PubMed]
  • Munk, M. H., Roelfsema, P. R., König, P., Engel, A. K., & Singer, W. (1996). Role of reticular activation in the modulation of intracortical synchronization.Science, 272, 271-274.[PubMed]
  • Neumann, O., & Scharlau, I. (in press). Experiments on the Fehrer-Raab effect and the 'Weather Station Model' of visual backward masking.Psychological Research.
  • Newman, J. (1995). Thalamic contributions to attention and consciousness.Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 172-193.[PubMed]
  • Otto, T. U., Öğmen, H., & Herzog, M. H. (2006, June).The visible trace of invisible elements.Paper presented at the workshop "Visual masking and the dynamics of vision and consciousness, Delmenhorst, Germany.
  • Purpura, D. (1970). Operations and processes in thalamic and synaptically related neural subsystems. In F. O. Schmitt (Ed.),The neurosciences. Second Study Program.(pp. 458-470). New York: Rockefeller University Press.
  • Purushothaman, G., Öğmen, H., & Bedell, H.E. (2000). Gamma-range oscillations in backward-masking functions and their putative neural correlates.Psychological Review, 107, 556-577.[PubMed]
  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Cobb, S. (1995). Visual attention modulates metacontrast masking.Nature, 373, 6-68.[PubMed]
  • Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 261-270.[PubMed]
  • Sherman, S. M., & Guillery, R. W. (1998). On the actions that one nerve cell can have on another: Distinguishing "drivers" from "modulators".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 7121-7126.[PubMed]
  • Singer, W. (1998). Consciousness and the structure of neuronal representations.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: B, 353, 1829-1840.[PubMed]
  • Singer, W., & Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis.Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 555-586.[PubMed]
  • Steriade, M. (1996a). Arousal: Revisiting the reticular activating system.Science, 272, 225-226.[PubMed]
  • Steriade, M. (1996b). Awakening the brain.Nature, 383, 24-25.[PubMed]
  • Steriade, M. (1997). Synchronized activities of coupled oscillators in the cerebral cortex and thalamus at different levels of vigilance.Cerebral Cortex, 7, 583-604.[PubMed]
  • Steriade, M. (2000). Corticothalamic resonance, states of vigilance and mentation.Neuroscience, 101, 243-276.[PubMed]
  • Steriade, M., Jones, E. G., & Llinás, R. R. (1990).Thalamic oscillations and signalling.New York: Wiley.
  • Steriade, M., Jones, E. G., & McCormick, D. A. (1997).Thalamus.Oxford: Elsevier.
  • Steriade, M., & McCarley, R. W. (2005).Brain control of wakefulness and sleep.New York: Springer / Klüwer / Plenum.
  • Summerfield, C., Jack, A. I., & Burgess, A. P. (2002). Induced gamma activity is associated with conscious awareness of pattern masked nouns.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 44, 93-100.
  • Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Henaff-Gonon, M.-A., Isnard, J., & Fischer, C. (2005). Differential attentional modulation of gamma-band oscillations in the human lateral occipital cortex and fusiform gyrus.Cerebral Cortex, 15, 654-662.
  • Taylor, K., Mandon, S., Freiwald, W.A., & Kreiter, A.K. (2005). Coherent oscillatory activity in monkey area V4 predicts successful allocation of attention.Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1424-1437.[PubMed]
  • Treisman, A. (1998). Feature binding, attention and object perception.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B, 353, 1295-1306.
  • Tremblay-Shelley, J., & Mack, A. (1999). Metacontrast masking and attention.Psychological Science, 10, 508-515.
  • VanRullen, R., & Koch, C. (2003). Competition and selection during visual processing of natural scenes and objects.Journal of Vision, 3, 75-85.
  • von der Malsburg, C. (1995). Binding in models of perception and brain function.Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 520-526.[PubMed]
  • Wang, W., & Slotine, J.-J. E. (2005). Fast computations with neural oscillators.Neurocomputing, 63, 7.
  • Ward, L. M. (2003). Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 553-559.[PubMed]
  • Womelsdorf, T., Fries, P., Mitra, P. P., & Desimone, R. (2006). Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection.Nature, 439, 733-736.[PubMed]
  • Di Lollo, V., Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of re-entrant visual processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 481-507.[PubMed]
  • Di Lollo, V., Lowe, D. G., & Scott, J. P., Jr. (1974). Backward masking and interference with the processing of brief visual displays.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 934-940.
  • Dixon, N. F. (1981).Preconscious processing.Chichester: Wiley.
  • Scharlau, I. (2004). Evidence against response bias in temporal order tasks with attention manipulation by masked primes.Psychological Research, 68, 224-236.[PubMed]
  • Scharlau, I., (in press). Perceptual latency priming: A measure of attentional facilitation.Psychological Research.
  • Scharlau, I., Ansorge, U., & Horstmann, G. (2005). Latency facilitation in temporal-order judgments: Time course of facilitation as a function of judgment type.Acta Psychologica, 122, 129-159.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0013-4
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.