Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2018 | 25/2 | 21-36

Article title

Effects of the Linguistic Context on Identity Relevant Self-Characterization in Bilinguals

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
A survey was conducted among Hungarian-Romanian-English multilinguals (N=237) with the aim of investigating the individual differences in cultural preferences and value priorities. Participants completed questionnaires measuring: (a) the individual level of national identification (Collective SelfEsteem); (b) own-culture preference (Patriotism); (c) ethnocentric proclivity toward other cultures (Generalized Ethnocentrism Scale); and, (d) the relative importance of 10 universal value types (Portrait Values Questionnaire) in three groups. Native (Hungarian) and acquired languages (Romanian, English) were used for filling out questionnaires for the three groups. Linguistic effects appeared on ethnocentrism, patriotism and collective aspects of self-esteem, but the effects were absent on personal aspect of self-esteem and prevailingly on value priorities. Results may indicate a kind of linguistic relativity in identity relevant self-characterization.

Year

Issue

Pages

21-36

Physical description

Dates

published
2018

Contributors

  • University of Debrecen
  • University of Debrecen
author
  • Babeş-Bolyai University
  • University of Debrecen

References

  • Au, T.K. (1983), Chinese and English counterfactuals: The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis revisited. In: Cognition 15, 155–187.
  • Bartha, C. (1999), A kétnyelvűség alapkérdései. Budapest.
  • Bartlett, F.C. (1932/1977), Remembering. A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge.
  • Chen, Y.R./ J. Brockner/ X.P. Chen (2002), Individual-collective primacy and ingroup favoritism: Enhancement and protection effects. In: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38, 482–491.
  • Chen, Y.R./ J. Brockner/ T. Katz (1998), Toward an explanation of cultural differences in in-group favoritism: The role of individual versus collective primacy. In: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75, 1490–1502.
  • Davidov, E. (2008), A cross-country and cross-time comparison of the human values measurements with the second round of the European Social Survey. In: Survey Research Methods 2, 33–46.
  • Ellis, N.C./ R.A. Hennely (1980), A bilingual word-length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. In: British Journal of Psychology 71, 43–52.
  • Fiske, S. (2003), Social beings: Core motives in social psychology. New York.
  • Greenwald, A. (2012), There is nothing so theoretical as a good method. In: Perspectives on Psychological Science 7 (2), 99–108.
  • Grosjean, F. (1992), Another view of bilingualism. In: R. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals. Amsterdam, 51–62.
  • Grosjean, F. (2008), Manipulating language mode. In: F. Grosjean (ed.), Studying bilinguals. Oxford, 67–84.
  • Grosjean, F. (2010), Bilingual: Life and Reality. Cambridge, Mass.
  • Hardin, C./ M.R. Banaji (1993), The influence of language on thought. In: Social Cognition 11 (3), 277–308.
  • Hoffman, C./ I. Lau/ D.R. Johnson (1986), The linguistic relativity of person cognition: An English-Chinese comparison. In: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51 (6), 1097–1105.
  • Hunt, E./ F. Agnoli (1991), The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychology perspective. In: Psychological Review, 98 (3), 377–389.
  • Kay, P./ W. Kempton (1984), What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? In: American Anthropologist 86 (1), 65–79.
  • Kinzler, K.D./ E. Dupoux/ E.S. Spleke (2007), The native language of social cognition. In: PNAS, 104 (30), 12577–12580.
  • Konde, Z./ G. Szabo/ Z. Dosa (2011), Investigating implicit attitudes toward historical memories. In: Applied Psychology in Hungary 13 (1), 22–40.
  • Kovacs, J./ J. Pantya/ A. Bernath/ D. Medves/ Zs. Banyai (2011), Subjects of Hungarian cultural memory in images. In: Applied Psychology in Hungary 13 (1), 7–21.
  • Kovacs, J./ J. Pantya/ D. Medves/ A. Bernath (2013), Communicating national heritage to an ingroup or to an outgroup member as a group or as an individual. In: P.S. Varga/ K. Katschthaler/ D.E. Morse/ M. Takacs (eds.), Loci Memoriae Hungaricae I. – The Theoretical Foundations of Hungarian 'lieux de mémoire' studies/ Theoretische Grundlagen der Erforschung ungarischer Erinnerungsorte. Debrecen, 137–150.
  • Leboeuf, R.A./ E. Shafir/ J.B. Bayuk (2010), The conflicting choices of alternating selves. In: Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 111, 48–61.
  • Loftus, E.F./ J.C. Palmer (1974), Reconstruction of automobile destruction: an example of the interaction between language and memory. In: Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13 (5), 585–589.
  • Luhtanen, R./ J. Crocker (1992), A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one's social identity. In: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18, 302–318.
  • Meier-Pesti, K./ E. Kirchler (2003), Nationalism and patriotism as determinants of European identity and attitudes towards the euro. In: The Journal of SocioEconomics 32, 685–700.
  • Miller, K.F./ D.R. Paredes (1996), On the shoulders of giants: Cultural tools and mathematical development. In: R.J. Sternberg./ T. Ben-Zeev (eds.), The nature of mathematical thinking. Mahwah, NJ, 83–117.
  • Mummendey, A./ A. Klink/ R. Brown (2001), Nationalism and patriotism: National identification and out-group rejection. In: British Journal of Social Psychology 40, 159–172.
  • Naveh-Benjamin, M./ T.J. Ayres (1986), Digits span, reading rate and linguistic relativity. In: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38, 339–351.
  • Neuliep J. W. (2002), Assessing the reliability and validity of the Generalized Ethnocentrism Scale. In: Journal of Intercultural Communication Research 31, 201–215.
  • Neuliep, J.W./ J. C. McCroskey (1997), The development of a U.S. and Generalized Ethnocentrism Scale. In: Communication Research Reports 14, 385–398.
  • Nosek, B.A./ A.G.Greenwald/ M.R. Banaji (2007), The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In: J.A. Bargh (ed.), Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior. New York, 265–292.
  • Ogunnaike, O./ Y. Dunham/ M.R. Banaji (2010), The language of implicit preferences. IN: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46, 999–1003.
  • Pinker, S. (1994), The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York.
  • Polonyi, T./ A. Kovacs (2005), Többnyelvű elmék. In: J. Gervain/ K. Kovacs/ A. Lukacs/ M. Racsmany (eds.), Ezerarcú elme. Budapest, 187–199.
  • Regier, T./ P. Kay/ L.G. Aubrey/ R.B. Ivry (2010), Language and thought: Which side are you on, anyway? In: B.C. Malt/ P. Wolff (eds.), Words and the mind. How words capture human experience. London, 165–182.
  • Schultz, P.W./ L.C. Zelazny (1998), Values and proenvironmental behavior: A fivecountry survey. In: Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 29, 540–558.
  • Schwartz, S.H. (1994), Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? In: Journal of Social Issues 50 (4), 19–45.
  • Schwartz, S.H. (2003), A proposal for measuring value orientations across nations. In: Chapter 7 in the questionnaire development package of the European Social Survey, date of access: 28.09.2014 http://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/index.php?option=com_docman&task=doc_view&gid=126&Itemid=80.
  • Schwartz, S.H. (2007), A theory of cultural value orientations: Explication and applications. In: Y. Esmer/ T. Pettersson (eds.), Measuring and mapping cultures: 25 years of comparative value surveys. Leiden, 33–78.
  • Schwartz, S.H./ G. Melech/ A. Lehmann/ S. Burgess/ M. Harris/ V. Owens (2001), Extending the cross-cultural validity of the theory of basic human values with a different method of measurement. In: Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 32, 519–542.
  • Takano, Y. (1989), Methodological problems in cross-cultural studies of linguistic relativity. In: Cognition 31 (2), 141–162.
  • Trafimow, D./ H.C. Triandis/ S.G. Goto (1991), Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. In: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60, 649–655.
  • Ybarra, O./ D. Trafimow (1998), How priming the private self or collective self affects the relative weights of attitudes and subjective norms. In: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24, 362–370.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

ISSN
2544-9354

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.desklight-794f573c-73ff-4518-8e86-266ca2228ab9
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.