Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2010 | 46 | 3 | 275-293

Article title

Mental Pollution Hypothesis and Foreign Vocabulary Retention

Selected contents from this journal

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
Research into second language acquisition lists several factors - social, psychological, cognitive, and affective - as reasons for the failure or low performance of many adult language learners. This present study was conducted to show that in addition to these factors it can also be greatly slowed by mental pollution - any ‘contamination of the mind’ by various forms of affective visual distractors, ranging in scope from violent or sexually suggestive images to comedic commercial advertisements and coming from a variety of media sources - from which younger learners are mostly protected but older learners are accustomed to encountering on an almost daily basis. In order to test the mental pollution hypothesis, 106 university students participated in the experiment which comprised vocabulary teaching along with two different treatments: mental pollutants being the first and free class time with relaxing music in the background comprising the second. The Equivalent Materials, Pretest, Posttest Design together, including descriptive statistics and paired sample t-tests, were used to analyze the vocabulary scores derived from the two treatments. The findings of the experiment indicate that when learners are exposed to high level of mental pollution, they demonstrate low retention of new vocabulary in a foreign language.

Publisher

Year

Volume

46

Issue

3

Pages

275-293

Physical description

Dates

published
2010-09-01
online
2010-09-22

Contributors

author
  • Fatih University, Istanbul
author
  • Fatih University, Istanbul

References

  • Ashcraft, M. H. and E. P. Kirk. 2001. "The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130. 224-237.
  • Au, S. 1988. "A critical appraisal of Gardner's social-psychological theory of second language (L2) learning". Language Learning 38. 75-100.
  • Baddeley, A. D. 2001. "Is working memory still working?" American Psychologist 56(11). 851-864.[Crossref]
  • Best, J. W. and J. V. Kahn. 2006. Research in education. (10th ed.) Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
  • Bialystok, E. 1997. "The structure of age: In search of barriers to second language acquisition". Second Language Research 13(2). 116-137.
  • Bongaerts, T., B. Planken and E. Schils. 1995. "Can late starters attain a native accent in a foreign language? A test of the critical period hypothesis". In: Singleton, D. and Z. Lengyel (eds.), The age factor in second language acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. 30-50.
  • Bushman, B. J. 2005. "Violence and sex in television programs do not sell products in advertisements". Psychological Science 16. 702-708.
  • Cardwell, M. and C. Flanagan. 2005. Psychology: The complete companion. Nelson Thornes.
  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1990. "Literacy and intrinsic motivation". Daedalus 119(2). 115-140.
  • Cook, V. 2001. Second language learning and language teaching. (3rd ed.) Arnold. London.
  • Cool, V. A., J. E. Patton, R. Runde and T. Z. Keith. 1994. "Experimental effects of radio and television distracters on children's performance on mathematics and reading assignments. Journal of Experimental Education, 62(3), 181-194.
  • Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 12. 769-786.[Crossref]
  • Cowan, N. 2001. "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24. 87-185.
  • Checkik, G., I. Meilijson, and E. Ruppin. 1997. "Synaptic pruning in development: A computational account". Neural Computation 10. 1759-1777.
  • Dinklage, K. 1971. "Inability to learn a foreign language". In: Blaine, G. and C. McArthur (eds.), Emotional problems of the student. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. 185-206.
  • Eckstrand, L. 1978. "Age and length of residence as variables related to the adjustment of migrant children with special reference to second language learning". In: Nickel, G. (ed.), Proceedings of the Fourth International Congress of Applied Linguistics. Stuttgart: Hochschulverlag. 179-197.
  • Ehrman, M. E. 1996. Understanding second language learning difficulties. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Entertainment Software Association (2004). Facts and research: Game player data. (Last accessed May 21,2005.)
  • Eysenck, H. 1978. Sex, violence, and the media. London: Maurice-Temple-Smith.
  • Fairbrother, N., S. Newth and S. Rachman. 2004. "Mental pollution: Feelings of dirtiness without physical contact". Behaviour Research and Therapy 43. 121-130.
  • Fenker, R. 1981. Stop studying start learning. Fort Worth, TX: Tangram.
  • Greenough, W. T. 1997. "We can't focus just on ages zero to three". APA Monitor 28. 19.
  • Hyltenstam, K. and N. Abrahamsson. 2003. "Maturational constraints in SLA". In: Doughty, C. J. and M. H. Long (eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition. Malden, MA: Blackwell. 539-588.
  • Joiner, E. G. 1981. The older foreign language learner: A challenge for colleges and universities. (Language in Education Series 34.) Washington, DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics.
  • Kahneman, D. 1973. Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Keith, T. Z. 1986. Homework. West Lafayette, IN: Kappa Delta Pi.
  • Krupski, A. 1980. "Attention processes: Research, theory, and implications for special education". In: Keough, B. K. (ed.), Advances in special education. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 101-140.
  • Kubey, R. and M. Csikszentmihalyi. 2002. "Television addiction is no mere metaphor". Scientific American 286. 74-80.
  • LeDoux, J. 2002. Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Viking Press.
  • Lee, D. and R. LaRose. 2007. "a socio-cognitive model of video game usage". Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media 51(4). 632-650.
  • Lesiuk, T. 2005."The effect of music listening on work performance". Psychology of Music 33(2). 173-191.[Crossref]
  • Lenneberg, E. H. 1967. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.
  • Lightbown, P. M. and N. Spada. 1999. How languages are learned. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Lin, H. and H. Thornburgh (eds.). 2002. Youth, pornography, and the internet. (Committee to Study Tools and Strategies for Protecting Kids from Pornography and Their Applicability to Other Inappropriate Internet Content.) Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
  • Kiefer, M., S. Schuch, W. Schenck and K. Fiedler. 2007. "Emotion and memory: Event-related potential indices predictive for subsequent successful memory depend on the emotional mood state". Advances in Cognitive Study 3(3). 363-373.
  • Marian, V. and U. Neisser. 2000. "Language-dependent recall of autobiographical memories". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129(3). 361-368.
  • McCraty, R. 1998. "The effects of different types of music on mood, tension, and mental clarity". Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine 4(1). 75-84.
  • Miller, P. H. and M. G. Weiss. 1982. "Children's and adults' knowledge about what variables affect selective attention". Child Development 53. 543-549.[Crossref]
  • Nelson, C. A. and F. E. Bloom. 1997. "Child development and neuroscience". Child Development 68(5). 970-987.[Crossref]
  • North, A. C., M. Tarrant and D. J. Hargreaves. 2004. "The effects of music on helping behavior: A field study". Environment and Behavior 36(2). 266-275.
  • Ostwald, S. K. and H. Y. Williams. 1985. "Optimizing learning in the elderly: A model". Lifelong Learning 9(10). 13-27.
  • Perani D. and J. Abutalebi. 2005. "The neural basis of first and second language processing". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15. 202-206.
  • Perry, R. A. 2004. "Foreign language acquisition and individual learner traits. A look at some spectacularly successful language learners". Revista de Ciencias Humanas 33.
  • Pimsleur, P. 1968. "Language aptitude testing". In: Davis, A. (ed.), Language testing symposium: A linguistic approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 98-106.
  • Robinson, M. 2009. Cupid's poisoned arrow: From habit to harmony in sexual relationships. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books.
  • Rosenfield, I. 1988. The invention of memory. A new view of the brain. New York: Basic Books.
  • Krashen, S. D. 1985. The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London: Longman.
  • Scovel, T. 1988. A tune to speak: A psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
  • Snow, C. E. and M. Hoefnagel-Hohle. 1978. "The critical period for language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning". Child Development 49. 1114-1128.
  • Sparks, R. and L. Ganschow. 1996. "Teachers perceptions of students' foreign language academic skills and affective characteristics". Journal of Educational Research 89. 172-185.
  • Weinberger, N. 2000. The Mozart effect: A small part of the big picture. (7th ed.) Sacramento: Regents of the University of California.
  • Williams, D. 2002. "A structural analysis of market competition in the U. S. home video game industry". International Journal on Media Management 4. 41-54.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_2478_v10010-010-0016-4
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.