Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2011 | 7 | 68-81

Article title

Roles of contour and surface processing in microgenesis of object perception and visual consciousness

Selected contents from this journal

Title variants

Languages of publication

Abstracts

EN
Developments in visual neuroscience and neural-network modeling indicate the existence of separate pathways for the processing of form and surface attributes of a visual object. In line with prior theoretical proposals, it is assumed that the processing of form can be explicit or conscious only as or after the surface property such as color is filled in. In conjunction with extant psychophysical findings, these developments point to interesting distinctions between nonconscious and conscious processing of these attributes, specifically in relation to distinguishable temporal dynamics. At nonconscious levels form processing proceeds faster than surface processing, whereas in contrast, at conscious levels form processing proceeds slower than surface processing. Implications of separate form and surface processing for current and future psychophysical and neuroscientific research, particularly that relating cortical oscillations to conjunctions of surface and form features, and for cognitive science and philosophy of mind and consciousness are discussed.

Year

Volume

7

Pages

68-81

Physical description

Contributors

author
  • Center for Neuro-Engineering and Cognitive Science, University of Houston, Houston, TX, USA

References

  • Arrington, K. F. (1994). The temporal dynamics of brightness filling-in.Vision Research, 34, 3371-3387.
  • Bar, M., & Biederman, I. (1999). Localizing the cortical region mediating visual awareness of object identity.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 96, 1790-1793.
  • Barbur, J. L., Harlow, A. J., & Plant, G. (1994). Insights into the different exploits of colour in the visual cortex.Proceedings of the Royal Society London B, 258, 327-334.
  • Beck, D. M., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness.Nature Neuroscience, 4, 645-650.
  • Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding.Psychological Review, 94, 115-147.
  • Biederman, I., & Ju, G. (1988). Surface versus edge-based determinants of visual recognition.Cognitive Psychology, 20, 38-64.
  • Boehler, C. N., Schoenfeld, M. A., Heinze, H.-J., & Hopf, J.-M. (2008). Rapid recurrent processing gates awareness in primary visual cortex.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105, 8742-8747.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G. (2007). Visual masking: Past accomplishments, present status, future developments.Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 9-20.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Herzog, M. H., & Öğmen, H. (2008). Motion, not masking, provides the medium for feature attribution.Psychological Science, 19, 823-829.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Kafaligönül, H., Öğmen, H., Mardon, L., Todd, S., & Ziegler, R. (2006). Meta- and paracontrast reveal differences between contour and brightness-processing mechanisms.Vision Research, 46, 2645-2658.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., & Öğmen, H. (2006).Visual masking: Time slices through conscious and unconscious vision.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Öğmen, H., & Chen, J. (2004). Unconscious priming by color and form: Different processes and levels.Conscious and Cognition, 13, 138-157.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Öğmen, H., Ramon, J., & Chen, J. (2005). Unconscious and conscious priming by forms and their parts.Visual Cognition, 12, 720-736.
  • Breitmeyer, B. G., & Stoerig, P. (2006). Neural correlates and levels of conscious and unconscious vision. In H. Öğmen & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.),The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes(pp. 35-48). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Broder, M., & Debruille, J. B. (2003). Visual evoked potentials to line- and luminance-defined triangles.Vision Research, 43, 299-306.
  • Bullier, J. (2001). Feedback connections and conscious vision.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 369-370.
  • Cao, Y., & Grossberg, S. (2005). A laminar cortical model of stereopsis and 3D surface perception: Closure and da Vinci stereopsis.Spatial Vision, 18, 515-578.
  • Caputo, G., Romani, A., Callieco, R., Gaspari, D., & Cosi, V. (1999). Amodal completion in texture visual evoked potentials.Vision Research, 39, 31-38.
  • Cohen, W. (1958). Color perception in the chromatic Ganzfeld.American Journal of Psychology, 71, 390-394.
  • Conway, B. R., Moeller, S., & Tsao, D. Y. (2007). Specialized color modules in macaque extrastriate cortex.Neuron, 56, 560-573.
  • Cowey, A., & Stoerig, P. (2001). Detection and discrimination of chromatic targets in hemianopic macaque monkeys and humans.European Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 1320-1330.
  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1995). Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex?Nature, 375, 121-123.
  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness.Nature Neuroscience, 6, 119-126.
  • Critchley, M. (1965). Acquired anomalies of colour perception of central origin.Brain, 88, 711-724.
  • Dehaene, S., Naccache, N., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.-F., Poline, J.-B., & Riviere, D. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming.Nature Neuroscience, 4, 752-758.
  • Desimone, R., Albright, T. D., Gross, C. G., & Bruce, C. (1984). Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque.Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2051-2062.
  • DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex.Trends in Neuroscience, 11, 219-226.
  • Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior.Science, 298, 1191-1194.
  • Elder, J. H., & Zucker, S. W. (1998). Evidence for boundary-specific grouping.Vision Research, 38, 3371-3387.
  • Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1993). Masking the face: Recognition of emotional facial expressions as a function of parameters of backward masking.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 34, 1-18.
  • Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1488-1497.
  • Felleman, D. J., Xiao, Y., & McClendon, E. (1997). Modular organization of occipito-temporal pathways: Cortical connections between visual area 4 and visual area 2 and posterior inferotemporal ventral area in macaque monkeys.Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 3185-3200.
  • Friedman, H. S., Zhou, H., & von der Heydt, R. (2003). The coding of uniform color figures in monkey visual cortex.Journal of Physiology, 548(2), 593-613.
  • Friedman-Hill, S. R., Robertson, L. C., & Treisman, A. (1997). Parietal contributions to visual feature binding: Evidence from a patient with bilateral lesions.Science, 269, 853-855.
  • Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of color vision.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 563-572.
  • Girard, P., Lomber, S. G., & Bullier, J. (2002). Shape discrimination deficits during reversible deactivation of area V4 in the macaque monkey.Cerebral Cortex, 12, 1146-1156.
  • Grossberg, S. (1987). Cortical dynamics of three-dimensional form, color, and brightness perception. I: Monocular theory.Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 87-116.
  • Grossberg, S. (1994). 3-Dvision and figure-ground separation by visual cortex.Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 48-120.
  • Grossberg, S. (2003). Filling-in the forms: Surface and boundary interactions in visual cortex. In L. Pessoa & P. De Weerd (Eds.),Filling-in: From perceptual completion to cortical reorganization(pp. 123-150). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Grossberg, S., & Swaminathan, G. (2004). A laminar cortical model for 3D perception of slanted and curved surfaces and of 2D images: Development, attention, and bistability.Vision Research, 44, 1147-1187.
  • Grossberg, S., & Yazdanbakhsh, A. (2005). Laminar cortical dynamics of 3D surface perception: Stratification, transparency, and neon color spreading.Vision Research, 45, 1725-1743.
  • Gur, M. (1989). Color and brightness fade-out in the Ganzfeld is wavelength dependent.Vision Research, 29, 1335-1341.
  • Herzog, M. H., & Koch, C. (2001). Seeing properties of an invisible object: Feature inheritance and shine through.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 4271-4275.
  • Heywood, C. A., Cowey, A., & Newcombe, F. (1991). Chromatic discrimination in a cortically colour blind observer.European Journal of Neuroscience, 3, 802-812.
  • Heywood, C. A., Gadotti, A., & Cowey, A. (1992). Cortical area V4 and its role in the perception of color.Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 4056-4065.
  • Heywood, C. A., Wilson, B., & Cowey, A. (1987). A case study of cortical colour "blindness" with relatively intact achromatic discrimination.Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 50, 22-29.
  • Hochberg, J. E., Triebel, W., & Seaman, G. (1951). Color adaptation under conditions of homogeneous visual stimulation (Ganzfeld).Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 153-159.
  • Hochstein, S., & Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system.Neuron, 36, 791-804.
  • Hong, S. W., & Blake, R. (2009). Interocular suppression differentially affects achromatic and chromatic mechanisms.Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 403-411.
  • Hong, S. W., & Shevell, S. K. (2006). Resolution of binocular rivalry: Perceptual misbinding of color.Visual Neuroscience, 23, 561-566.
  • Hubel, D. H. (1988).Eye, brain, and vision.New York: W. H. Freeman.
  • Hummel, J. E., & Biederman, I. (1992). Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition.Psychological Review, 99, 480-517.
  • Humphreys, G. W., Cinel, C., Wolfe, J., Olson, A., & Klempen, N. (2000). Fractioning the binding process: Neurophsychological evidence distinguishing binding of form from binding of surface features.Vision Research, 40, 1569-1596.
  • Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1994). Attention to within-object and between-object spatial representations: Multiple sites for visual selection.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 207-241.
  • Hyman, J. (2006).The objective eye: Colour, form, and reality in the theory of art.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Johnson, E. N., Hawken, M. J., & Shapley, R. (2001). The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey.Nature Neuroscience, 4, 409-416.
  • Kahneman, D. (1968). Method, findings, and theory in studies of visual masking.Psychological Bulletin, 70, 404-425.
  • Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Cowey, A. (2004). Chromatic edges, surfaces, and constancies in cerebral achromatopsia.Neuropsychologia, 42, 821-830.
  • Kim, C.-Y., & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: Rendering the visible "invisible".Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 381-388.
  • Kinoshita, M., & Komatsu, H. (2001). Neural representation of luminance an brightness of a uniform surface in the macaque primary visual cortex.Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 2559-2570.
  • Kiper, D. C. (2003). Colour and form in the early stages of cortical processing.Journal of Physiology, 548(2), 335.
  • Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 976-992.
  • Klotz, W., & Wolff, P. (1995). The effect of a masked stimulus on the response to the masking stimulus.Psychological Research, 58, 92-101.
  • Koch, C. (2004).The quest for consciousness.Englewood, CO: Roberts & Co.
  • Komatsu, H., Ideura, Y., Kaji, S., & Yamane, S. (1992). Color selectivity in neurons in the inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque monkey.Journal of Neursocience, 12, 408-424.
  • Lamme, V. A. F. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 494-501.
  • Lamme, V. A. F., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, V., & Spekreijse, H. (1999). Separate processing dynamics for texture elements, boundaries, and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey.Cerebral Cortex, 9, 406-413.
  • Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing.Trends in Neuroscience, 23, 571-579.
  • Lee, T., Mumford, D., & Schiller, P. (1995). Neuronal correlates of boundary and medial axis representations in primate visual cortex.Journal of Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 36, 477.
  • Leventhal, A. G., Thompson, K. G., Liu, D., Zhou, Y., & Ault, S. (1995). Concomitant sensitivity to orientation, direction, and color of cells in layers 2, 3, and 4 of monkey striate cortex.Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1808-1818.
  • Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form color, movement, and depth.Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 3416-3468.
  • Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception.Science, 240, 740-749.
  • Lu, H. D., & Roe, A. W. (2008). Functional organization of color domains in V1 and V2 of macaque monkey revealed by optical imaging.Cerebral Cortex, 18, 516-533.
  • Lumer, E. D., Friston, K. J., & Rees, G. (1998). Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain.Science, 280, 1930-1934.
  • Marr, D. (1982).Vision.San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
  • Marr, D., & Nishihara, H. K. (1978). Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 200, 269-294.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995).The visual brain in action.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems reviewed.Neuropsychologia, 46, 774-785.
  • Morsella, E. (2005). The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory.Psychological Review, 112, 1000-1021.
  • Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997). Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive system.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 264, 1407-1414.
  • Neumann, O., & Klotz, W. (1994). Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli. Where is the limit of direct parameter specification? In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.),Attention and performance XV(pp. 123-150). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Öğmen, H., Breitmeyer, B. G., & Melvin, R. (2003). The what and where in visual masking.Vision Research, 43, 1337-1350.
  • Öğmen, H., Breitmeyer, B. G., Todd, S., & Mardon, L. (2006). Target recovery in metacontrast: The effect of contrast.Vision Research, 46, 4726-4734.
  • Pasupathy, A., & Connor, C. E. (2002). Population coding of shape in area V4.Nature Neuroscience, 5, 1332-1338.
  • Persaud, N., & Cowey, A. (2008). Blindsight is unlike normal conscious vision: Evidence from an exclusion task.Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 1050-1055.
  • Pessoa, L., & De Weerd, P. (2003).Filling-in: From perceptual completion to cortical reorganization.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Pollen, D. A. (2008). Fundamental requirements for primary visual perception.Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1991-1998.
  • Pöppel, E., Held, R., & Frost, D. (1973). Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man.Nature, 243, 295-296.
  • Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanism of consciousness.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91, 7398-7403.
  • Ramachandran, V. S. (1992). Blind spots.Scientific American, 266, 86-91.
  • Ramachandran, V. S. (2003). Foreword. In L. Pessoa & P. De Weerd (Eds.),Filling-in: From perceptual completion to cortical reorganization(pp. xi-xxii). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Rees, G. (2008). Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in humans.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 877-886.
  • Robertson, L. C. (2003). Binding, spatial attention, and perceptual awareness.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 93-102.
  • Roe, A. W., & Ts'o, D. Y. (1999). Specificity of color connectivity between primate V1 and V2.Journal of Neurophysiology, 82, 2719-2730.
  • Roelfsema, P. R., Lamme, V. A. F., Spekreijse, H., & Bosch, H. (2002). Figure-ground segregation in a recurrent network architecture.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 525-537.
  • Rogers-Ramachandran, D. C., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1998). Psychophysical evidence for boundary and surface systems in human vision.Vision Research, 38, 71-77.
  • Romani, A., Caputo, G., Callieco, R., Schintone, E., & Cosi, V. (1999). Edge detection and surface "filling in" as shown by texture visual evoked potentials.Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 86-91.
  • Rossi, A. F., & Paradiso, M. A. (2003). Surface completion: Psychophysical and neurophysiological studies of brightness interpolation. In L. Pessoa & P. De Weerd (Eds.),Filling-in: From perceptual completion to cortical reorganization(pp. 59-80). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Scheinberg, D. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (1997). The role of temporal visual areas in perceptual organization.Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 94, 3408-3413.
  • Scholte, H. S., Jolij, J., Fahrenfort, J. J., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2008). Feed forward and recurrent processing in scene segmentation: Electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance imaging.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2097-2109.
  • Searle, J. (1992).The rediscovery of mind.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Shevell, S. K., St. Clair, R., & Hong, S. W. (2008). Misbinding of color in afterimages.Visual Neuroscience, 25, 355-360.
  • Sincich, L. C., & Horton, J. C. (2005a). Input to V2 thin stripes arises from V1 cytochrome oxidase patches.Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 10087-10093.
  • Sincich, L. C., & Horton, J. C. (2005b). The circuitry of V1 and V2: Integration of color, form, and motion.Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 303-326.
  • Stoerig, P. (1996). Varieties of vision: From blind responses to conscious recognition.Trends in Neuroscience, 19, 401-406.
  • Stoerig, P. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness as state and trait. In L. Nadel (Ed.),Encyclopaedia of Cognitive Neuroscience(pp. 233-240). London: MacMillan.
  • Sun, P., Ueno, K., Waggoner, R. L., Gardener, J. L., Tanaka, K., & Cheng, K. (2007). A temporal frequency-dependent functional architecture in human V1 revealed by high resolution fMRI.Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1404-1406.
  • Supèr, H., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2001). Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1).Nature Neuroscience, 4, 304-310.
  • Tanaka, K., Saito, H., Fukuda, Y., & Moriya, M. (1991). Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex.Journal of Neurophysiology, 66, 170-189.
  • Thorell, L. G., De Valois, R. L., & Albrecht, D. G. (1984). Spatial mapping of monkey V1 cells with pure color and luminance stimuli.Vision Research, 24, 751-769.
  • Treisman, A. (1988). Feature binding, attention, and object perception.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 353, 1295-1306.
  • Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages.Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1096-1101.
  • Tsunoda, K., Yamane, Y., Nishizaki, M., & Tanifuji, M. (2001). Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns.Nature Neuroscience, 4, 832-838.
  • Ullman, S. (1984). Visual routines.Cognition, 18, 97-159.
  • Ullman, S. (1996).High-level vision: Object recognition and visual cognition.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • VanRullen, R. (2007). The power of the feed-forward sweep.Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3, 167-176.
  • Veser, S., O'Shea, R. P., Schröger, E., Trujillo-Barreto, N. J., & Roeber, U. (2008). Early correlates of visual awareness following orientation and colour rivalry.Vision Research, 48, 2359-2369.
  • Viviani, P., & Aymoz, C. (2001). Colour, form, and movement are not perceived simultaneously.Vision Research, 41, 2909-2918.
  • Walsh, V., Carden, D., Butler, S. R., & Kulikowski, J. J. (1993). The effects of V4 lesions on the visual abilities of macaques: Hue discrimination and color constancy.Behavioral Brain Research, 53, 51-62.
  • Wang, Y., Xiao, Y., & Felleman, D. J. (2007). V2 thin stripes contain spatially organized representations of achromatic luminance change.Cerebral Cortex, 17, 116-129.
  • Weiskrantz, L. (1997).Consciousness lost and found: A neuropsychological exploration.Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Wiesenfelder, H., & Blake, R. (1991). Apparent motion can survive binocular rivalry suppression.Vision Research, 31, 1589-1599.
  • Xiao, Y., Casti, A., Xiao, J., & Kaplan, E. (2007). A spatially organized representation of colour in macaque cortical area V2.NeuroImage, 35, 771-786.
  • Xiao, Y., & Felleman, D. J. (2004). Projections from primary visual cortex to cytochrome oxidase thin stripes and interstripes of macaque visual area 2.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 792-804.
  • Xiao, Y., Wang, Y., & Felleman, D. J. (2003). A spatially organized representation of colour in macaque cortical area V2.Nature, 421, 535-539.
  • Xiao, Y., Zych, A., & Felleman, D. J. (1999). Segregation and convergence of functionally defined V2 thin stripe and interstripe compartments projections to area V4 of macaques.Cerebral Cortex, 9, 792-804.
  • Zeki, S. (1983a). Color coding in cerebral cortex: The reaction of cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition.Neuroscience, 9, 741-765.
  • Zeki, S. (1983b). Color coding in cerebral cortex: The reaction of wavelength-selective and color-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition.Neuroscience, 9, 767-781.
  • Zeki, S. (2005). Behind the seen: The functional specialization of the brain (The Ferrier Lecture 1995).Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 360, 1145-1183.
  • Zeki, S., Aglioti, S., McKeefry, D., & Berlucchi, G. (1999). The neurological basis of conscious color perception in a blind patient.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 96, 14124-14129.
  • Zeki, S., & Bartels, A. (1998). The asynchrony of consciousness.Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 265, 1583-1585.
  • Zeki, S., & Marini, L. (1998). Three cortical stages of colour processing in the human brain.Brain, 121, 1669-1685.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.cejsh-article-doi-10-2478-v10053-008-0088-y
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.