Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2019 | 22 | 4 | 373-403

Article title

What do we actually measure as music-induced emotions?

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
The paper presents the results of a systematic review of 61 empirical studies in which emotions in response to music were measured. The analysis of each study was focused on the measurement of emotion components and the conceptualization of emotion both in hypothesis and discussion. The review does not support the claim that music evokes the same emotional reactions as life events do, especially modal emotions. Notably, neither a high intensity of feelings, nor intentionality were confirmed in relation to musical experiences, the emergence of specific action tendencies, or specific physiological changes. Based on the obtained results, it is recommended to use the terms “affect” or “music emotions” with reference to emotions experienced in reaction to music and to abandon the term “emotions” as misleading.

Year

Volume

22

Issue

4

Pages

373-403

Physical description

Contributors

  • Uniwersytet Śląski w Katowicach
  • Uniwersytet Śląski w Katowicach

References

  • Balteş, F. R., Avram, J., Miclea, M., & Miu, A. C. (2011). Emotions induced by operatic music: Psychophysiological effects of music, plot, and acting. Brain and Cognition, 76(1), 146–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.01.012
  • Balteş, F. R., Miclea, M., & Miu, A. C. (2012). Does everybody like Vivaldi’s Four Seasons? Affective space and a comparison of music-induced emotions between musicians and non-musicians. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 16(1), 107–119.
  • Balteș, F. R., & Miu, A. C. (2014). Emotions during live music performance: Links with individual differences in empathy, visual imagery, and mood. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 24(1), 58–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000030
  • Barrett, L. F. (1998). Discrete emotions or dimensions? The role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cognition & Emotion, 12(4), 579–599. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379574
  • Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 20–46. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2
  • Bicknell, J. (2007). Explaining strong emotional responses to music. Sociality and intimacy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(12), 5–23.
  • Bigand, E., Vieillard, S., Madurell, F., Marozeau, J., & Dacquet, A. (2005). Multidimensional scaling of emotional responses to music: The effect of musical expertise and of the duration of the excerpts. Cognition & Emotion, 19(8), 1113–1139. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500204250
  • Briesemeister, B. B., Kuchinke, L., & Jacobs, A. M. (2011). Discrete emotion effects on lexical decision response times. PLoS ONE, 6(8), e23743. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0023743
  • Cespedes-Guevara, J., & Eerola, T. (2018). Music communicates affects, not basic emotions—A constructionist account of attribution of emotional meanings to music. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 215. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00215
  • Chełkowska-Zacharewicz, M. (2018). Preferences for different emotional response scale types in music experience. Poster presented at the 15th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition and at the 10th triennial conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music in Graz, Austria.
  • Cheng, F.-F., Wu, C.-S., & Yen, D. C. (2009). The effect of online store atmosphere on consumer’s emotional responses—An experimental study of music and colour. Behaviour & Information Technology, 28(4), 323–334. https://doi.org/10.1080/01449290701770574
  • Choppin, S., Trost, W., Dondaine, T., Millet, B., Drapier, D., Vérin, M., Robert, G., & Grandjean, D. (2016). Alteration of complex negative emotions induced by music in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 15–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.10.063
  • Corrigall, K. A., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2013). Music: The language of emotion. In C. Mohiyeddini, M. Eysenck, & S. Bauer (Eds.), Psychology of emotions, motivations and actions. Handbook of psychology of emotions: Recent theoretical perspectives and novel empirical findings (vol. 2, pp. 299–325). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.
  • Coutinho, E., & Scherer, K. R. (2017). Introducing the GEneva Music-Induced Affect Checklist (GEMIAC): A brief instrument for the rapid assessment of musically induced emotions. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 34(4), 371–386. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2017.34.4.371
  • Daly, I., Malik, A., Hwang, F., Roesch, E., Weaver, J., Kirke, A., Williams, D., Miranda, E., & Nasuto, S. J. (2014). Neural correlates of emotional responses to music: An EEG study. Neuroscience Letters, 573, 52–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2014.05.003
  • Daly, I., Williams, D., Hallowell, J., Hwang, F., Kirke, A., Malik, A., Weaver, J., Miranda, E., & Nasuto, S. J. (2015). Music-induced emotions can be predicted from a combination of brain activity and acoustic features. Brain and Cognition, 101, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.08.003
  • Damasio, A. R. (2005). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. London: Penguin.
  • Davidson, R. J. (1994). Complexities in the search for emotion-specific physiology. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 237–242). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
  • Dibben, N. (2004). The role of peripheral feedback in emotional experience with music. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22(1), 79–115. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2004.22.1.79
  • Eerola, T., & Vuoskoski, J. K. (2011). A comparison of the discrete and dimensional models of emotion in music. Psychology of Music, 39(1), 18–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610362821
  • Eerola, T., & Vuoskoski, J. K. (2013). A review of music and emotion studies: Approaches, emotion models, and stimuli. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(3), 307–340. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2012.30.3.307
  • Eerola, T., Vuoskoski, J. K., & Kautiainen, H. (2016). Being moved by unfamiliar sad music is associated with high empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1176. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01176
  • Egermann, H., & McAdams, S. (2013). Empathy and emotional contagion as a link between recognized and felt emotions in music listening. Music Perception, 31(2), 139–156. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2013.31.2.139
  • Egermann, H., Nagel, F., Altenmüller, E., & Kopiez, R. (2009). Continuous measurement of musically-induced emotion: A web experiment. International Journal of Internet Science, 4(1), 4–20.
  • Egermann, H., Sutherland, M. E., Grewe, O., Nagel, F., Kopiez, R., & Altenmüller, E. (2011). Does music listening in a social context alter experience? A physiological and psychological perspective on emotion. Musicae Scientiae, 15(3), 307–323. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864911399497
  • Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200.
  • Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 4(48), 384–392.
  • Ekman, P. (1994). Moods, emotions, and traits. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 56–58). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
  • Ekman, P. (1999). Facial expressions. In T. Power & P. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 301-320). Chichester, UK, & New York, NY, US: Wiley.
  • Etzel, J. A., Johnsen, E. L., Dickerson, J., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Cardiovascular and respiratory responses during musical mood induction. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(1), 57–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.025
  • Evans, P., & Schubert, E. (2008). Relationships between expressed and felt emotions in music. Musicae Scientiae, 12(1), 75–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/102986490801200105
  • Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(3), 464–486.
  • Frijda, N. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43(5), 349–358.
  • Frijda, N. (1994). Varieties of affect: Emotions and episodes, moods, and sentiments. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion, fundamental questions (pp. 59–67). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotion perceived and emotion felt: Same or different?. Musicae Scientiae, Spec Issue 2001–2002, 123–147. https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649020050S105
  • Gabrielsson, A. (2011). Strong experiences with music: Music is much more than just music. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gabrielsson, A., & Wik, S. L. (2003). Strong experiences related to music: A descriptive system. Musicae Scientiae, 7(2), 157–217. https://doi.org/10.1177/102986490300700201
  • Gingras, B., Marin, M. M., Puig-Waldmüller, E., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). The eye is listening: Music-induced arousal and individual differences predict pupillary responses. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 619. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00619
  • Goldsmith, H. H. (1994). Parsing the emotional domain from a development perspective. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 68–73). New York, NY, US: Oxford University.
  • Gomez, P., & Danuser, B. (2007). Relationships between musical structure and psychophysiological measures of emotion. Emotion, 7(2), 377–387. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.377
  • Heller, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Mayer, K. E., & Davidson, R. J. (2014). The face of negative affect: Trial-by-trial corrugator responses to negative pictures are positively associated with amygdala and negatively associated with ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(9), 2102–2110. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00622
  • Hunter, P. G., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). Music and emotion. In M. Riess Jones, R. R. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Music perception (vol. 36, pp. 129–164). New York, NY, US: Springer New York.
  • Hunter, P. G., Schellenberg, E. G., & Schimmack, U. (2010). Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music: Similarities, differences, and mixed emotions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(1), 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016873
  • Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 260–280. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00044.x
  • Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163539
  • Jaquet, L., Danuser, B., & Gomez, P. (2014). Music and felt emotions: How systematic pitch level variations affect the experience of pleasantness and arousal. Psychology of Music, 42(1), 51–70. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612456583
  • Johnsen, E. L., Tranel, D., Lutgendorf, S., & Adolphs, R. (2009). A neuroanatomical dissociation for emotion induced by music. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(1), 24–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.011
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1988). Are there only two primitive emotions? A reply to Frijda. Cognition & Emotion, 2(2), 89–93.
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1989). The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cognition & Emotion, 3(2), 81–123.
  • Juslin P. N., Barradas, G., & Eerola, T. (2015). From sound to significance: Exploring the mechanisms underlying emotional reactions to music. The American Journal of Psychology, 128(3), 281–304. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.3.0281
  • Juslin, P., Harmat, L., & Eerola, T. (2014). What makes music emotionally significant? Exploring the underlying mechanisms. Psychology of Music, 42(4), 599–623. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613484548
  • Juslin, P., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., & Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling study of emotional reactions to music: Listener, music, and situation. Emotion, 8(5), 668–683. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013505
  • Juslin, P. N., & Västfjäll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(05), 559–621. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X08005293
  • Kawakami, A., Furukawa, K., Katahira, K., & Okanoya, K. (2013). Sad music induces pleasant emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(June), 311. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00311
  • Kawakami, A., Kiyoshi, F., Katahira, K., Kamiyama, K., & Okanoya, K. (2013). Relations between musical structures and perceived and felt emotions. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(4), 407–417. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2013.30.4.407
  • Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 13(5), 467–480.
  • Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2001). Social functions of emotions. In T. Mayne & G. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 192–213). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
  • Kolańczyk, A., Fila-Jankowska, A., Pawłowska-Fusiara, M., & Sterczyński, R. (2004). Serce w rozumie: afektywne podstawy orientacji w otoczeniu [Heart in mind: The affective basis of orientation in the environment]. Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.
  • Konečni, V. J. (2008). A skeptical position on “musical emotions” and an alternative proposal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(05). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X08005372
  • Konečni, V. J. (2011). Aesthetic trinity theory and the sublime. Philosophy Today, 55(1), 64–73. https/doi.org/10.5840/philtoday201155162
  • Konečni, V. J. (2013). Music, affect, method, data: Reflections on the Carroll versus Kivy debate. The American Journal of Psychology, 126(2), 179–195. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.2.0179
  • Kopec, J., Hillier, A., & Frye, A. (2014). The valency of music has different effects on the emotional responses of those with autism spectrum disorders and a comparison group. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 31(5), 436-443. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2014.31.5.436
  • Kowalska, M., & Wróbel, M. (2017). Basic emotions. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences (pp. 1–6). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
  • Krahé, C., Hahn, U., & Whitney, K. (2015). Is seeing (musical) believing? The eye versus the ear in emotional responses to music. Psychology of Music, 43(1), 140–148. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613498920
  • Kreutz, G., Ott, U., Teichmann, D., Osawa, P., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Using music to induce emotions: Influences of musical preference and absorption. Psychology of Music, 36(1), 101–126. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607082623
  • Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 51(4), 336–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1196-1961.51.4.336
  • Labbé, C., Glowinski, D., & Grandjean, D. (2016). The effect of ensemble and solo performance on affective experiences induced by music. Psychology of Music, 45(4), 600–608. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735616659551
  • Labbé, C., & Grandjean, D. (2014). Musical emotions predicted by feelings of entrainment. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 32(2), 170–185. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2014.32.2.170
  • Ladinig, O., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2012). Liking unfamiliar music: Effects of felt emotion and individual differences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(2), 146–154. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024671
  • Larsen, J. T., Norris, C. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Effects of positive and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii. Psychophysiology, 40(5), 776-785.
  • Larsen, J. T., & Stastny, B. J. (2011). It’s a bittersweet symphony: Simultaneously mixed emotional responses to music with conflicting cues. Emotion, 11(6), 1469–1473. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024081
  • Lindquist, K. A., Siegel, E. H., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). The hundred-year emotion war: Are emotions natural kinds or psychological constructions? Comment on Lench, Flores, and Bench (2011). Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 255–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029038
  • Lundqvist, L.-O., Carlsson, F., Hilmersson, P., & Juslin, P. N. (2009). Emotional responses to music: Experience, expression, and physiology. Psychology of Music, 37(1), 61–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607086048
  • Marin, M. M., Gingras, B., & Bhattacharya, J. (2012). Crossmodal transfer of arousal, but not pleasantness, from the musical to the visual domain. Emotion, 12(3), 618–631. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025020
  • Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M., Frank, M., & O’Sullivan, M. (2008). What’s in a face? Facial expressions as signals of discrete emotions. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 211–234). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
  • Miu, A. C., & Balteş, F. R. (2012). Empathy manipulation impacts music-induced emotions: A psychophysiological study on opera. PLoS ONE, 7(1), e30618. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030618
  • Mori, K., & Iwanaga, M. (2014). Pleasure generated by sadness: Effect of sad lyrics on the emotions induced by happy music. Psychology of Music, 42(5), 643–652. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613483667
  • Naji, M., Firoozabadi, M., & Azadfallah, P. (2014). Classification of music-induced emotions based on information fusion of forehead biosignals and electrocardiogram. Cognitive Computation, 6(2), 241–252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559-013-9239-7
  • Olsen, K. N., & Stevens, C. J. (2013). Psychophysiological response to acoustic intensity change in a musical chord. Journal of Psychophysiology, 27(1), 16–26. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000082
  • Pesek, M., Strle, G., Kavčič, A., & Marolt, M. (2017). The Moodo dataset: Integrating user context with emotional and color perception of music for affective music information retrieval. Journal of New Music Research, 46(3), 246–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/09298215.2017.1333518
  • Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145–172. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.110.1.145
  • Sacharin, V., Schlegel, K., & Scherer, K. R. (2012). Geneva Emotion Wheel rating study (Report). University of Geneva, Swiss Center for Affective Sciences.
  • Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14(2), 257–262. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2726
  • Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Longo, G., Cooperstock, J. R., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). The rewarding aspects of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal. PLoS ONE, 4(10), e7487. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007487
  • Sammler, D., Grigutsch, M., Fritz, T., & Koelsch, S. (2007). Music and emotion: Electrophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant and unpleasant music. Psychophysiology, 44(2), 293–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00497.x
  • Scherer, K. R. (2004). Which emotions can be induced by music? What are the underlying mechanisms? And how can we measure them? Journal of New Music Research, 33(3), 239–251. https://doi.org/10.1080/0929821042000317822
  • Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Social Science Information, 44(4), 695–729. https://doi.org/10.1177/053901840505821
  • Scherer, K. R., Shuman, V., Fontaine, J. R. J., & Soriano, C. (2013). The GRID meets the Wheel: Assessing emotional feeling via self-report1. In J. R. J. Fontaine, K. R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 281–298). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199592746.003.0019
  • Scherer, K. R., Trznadel, S., Fantini, B., & Coutinho, E. (2019). Assessing emotional experiences of opera spectators in situ. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13(3), 244–258. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000163 [Access online first].
  • Schubert, E. (2007). The influence of emotion, locus of emotion and familiarity upon preference in music. Psychology of Music, 35(3), 499–515. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607072657
  • Schubert, E. (2010). Affective, evaluative, and collative responses to hated and loved music. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(1), 36–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016316
  • Schubert, E. (2013). Loved music can make a listener feel negative emotions. Musicae Scientiae, 17(1), 11–26. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864912461321
  • Shaver, P. R., Morgan, H. J., & Wu, S. (1996). Is love a “basic” emotion? Personal Relationships, 3(1), 81–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1996.tb00105.x
  • Shaver, P. R., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1061–1086. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.52.6.1061
  • Song, Y., Dixon, S., Pearce, M. T., & Halpern, A. R. (2016). Perceived and induced emotion responses to popular music: Categorical and dimensional models. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 33(4), 472–492. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2016.33.4.472
  • Tabei, K. (2015). Inferior frontal gyrus activation underlies the perception of emotions, while precuneus activation underlies the feeling of emotions during music listening. Behavioural Neurology, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/529043
  • Tan, J.-W., Walter, S., Scheck, A., Hrabal, D., Hoffmann, H., Kessler, H., & Traue, H. C. (2011). Facial electromyography (fEMG) activities in response to affective visual stimulation. 2011 IEEE Workshop on Affective Computational Intelligence (WACI), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1109/WACI.2011.5953144
  • Thompson, W. F., Geeves, A. M., & Olsen, K. N. (2019). Who enjoys listening to violent music and why? Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 8(3), 218–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000184 [Access online first].
  • Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2015). Corrugator activity confirms immediate negative affect in surprise. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 134. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00134
  • Trost, W., Ethofer, T., Zentner, M., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Mapping aesthetic musical emotions in the brain. Cerebral Cortex, 22(12), 2769–2783. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr353
  • Trost, W., Frühholz, S., Cochrane, T., Cojan, Y., & Vuilleumier, P. (2015). Temporal dynamics of musical emotions examined through intersubject synchrony of brain activity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(12), 1705–1721. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv060
  • Vásquez-Rosati, A. (2017). Body awareness to recognize feelings. Constructivist Foundations, 12(2), 219-226.
  • Vernooij, E., Orcalli, A., Fabbro, F., & Crescentini, C. (2016). Listening to the Shepard-Risset glissando: The relationship between emotional response, disruption of equilibrium, and personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 300. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00300
  • Vuoskoski, J. K., & Eerola, T. (2011). Measuring music-induced emotion: A comparison of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences. Musicae Scientiae, 15(2), 159–173. https://doi.org/10.1177/102986491101500203
  • Vuoskoski, J. K., & Eerola, T. (2017). The pleasure evoked by sad music is mediated by feelings of being moved. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 439. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00439
  • Vuoskoski, J. K., Gatti, E., Spence, C., & Clarke, E. F. (2016). Do visual cues intensify the emotional responses evoked by musical performance? A psychophysiological investigation. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 26(2), 179–188. https://doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000142
  • Vuoskoski, J. K., Thompson, W. F., McIlwain, D., & Eerola, T. (2012). Who enjoys listening to sad music and why? Music Perception, 29(3), 311–317. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2012.29.3.311
  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Emotions, moods, traits, and temperaments: Conceptual distinctions and empirical findings. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 89–93). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
  • Weth, K., Raab, M. H., & Carbon, C.-C. (2015). Investigating emotional responses to self-selected sad music via self-report and automated facial analysis. Musicae Scientiae, 19(4), 412–432. https://doi.org/10.1177/1029864915606796
  • Wheeler, B. L., Sokhadze, E., Baruth, J., Behrens, G. A., & Quinn, C. F. (2011). Musically induced emotions: Subjective measures of arousal and valence. Music and Medicine, 3(4), 224–233. https://doi.org/10.1177/1943862111407513
  • Wu, X., & Sun, G. (2018). Music-induced emotions and musical regulation and emotion improvement based on EEG technology. NeuroQuantology, 16(6), 379–384. https://doi.org/10.14704/nq.2018.16.6.1593
  • Yamauchi, T., & Xiao, K. (2018). Reading emotion from mouse cursor motions: Affective computing approach. Cognitive Science, 42(3), 771–819. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12557
  • Yik, M., Russell, J. A., & Steiger, J. H. (2011). A 12-point circumplex structure of core affect. Emotion, 11(4), 705–731. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023980
  • Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion, 8(4), 494–521. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.desklight-15a00a9a-563b-4cd0-a04c-b74c57226a26
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.