Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2015 | 19 | 1 | 1-18

Article title

The Associative Structure of the Mental Lexicon: Hierarchical Semantic Relations in the Minds of Blind and Sighted Language Users

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
This paper concerns the role of hierarchical semantic relations: class inclusion and partwhole relations as factors organising the mental lexicon, and the dependence of their importance on visual perception and visual memories, as demonstrated by the results of a free association task. 58 blind and 58 sighted language users were instructed to give associations for a list of 75 Polish nouns. Semantic analysis showed that more than 40% of the whole corpus of answers was related to stimuli through the part-whole or class inclusion relations. The results of the analysis indicated many similarities, concerning both types of relations, in the feedback obtained from the blind and sighted respondents. However, the blind participants showed a significantly stronger tendency to respond with inclusive terms (hyperonyms of the stimuli) than the sighted respondents. Th e results were interpreted in terms of the specificity of the compensation processes.

Publisher

Year

Volume

19

Issue

1

Pages

1-18

Physical description

Dates

published
2015-05-01
online
2015-05-29

Contributors

  • Institute of Linguistics, Faculty of Modern Languages and Literature, Adam Mickiewicz University, Al. Niepodległości 4, 61-874 Poznań, Poland

References

  • Aitchison, J. (2003). Words in the Mind. An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  • Andersen, E.S., Dunlea, A., & Kekelis, L.S. (1984). Blind children’s language: Resolving some differences. Journal of Child Language, 11 (3), 645-664.
  • Bloom, P. (2000). How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Bock, J. K. (2002). Meaning, sound, and syntax. Lexical priming in sentence production. In G.T.M. Altmann (Ed.), Psycholinguistics. Critical Concepts in Psychology. Vol. 5 (pp. 378-404). London, New York: Routledge.
  • Chaffin, R. (1992). The concept of a semantic relation. In A. Lehrer & A.F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, Fields and Contrasts (pp. 253-288). Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Chaffin, R. & Herrmann, D.J. (1984). The similarity and diversity of semantic relations. Memory and Cognition, 12 (2), 134-141.[PubMed]
  • Chaffin, R. & Herrmann, D.J. (1987). Relation element theory: a new account of the representation and processing of semantic relations. In D.S. Gorfein & R.R. Hoffman (Eds.), Memory and Learning. The Ebbinghaus Centennial Conference (pp. 221-245). Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Chaffin, R. & Herrmann, D.J. (1988). The nature of semantic relations: A comparison of two approaches. In M.W. Evens (Ed.), Relational Models of the Lexicon. Representing Knowledge in Semantic Networks (pp. 289-334). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Collins, A.M. & Qu illian, M.R. (1972). How to make a language user. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of Memory (pp. 309-351). New York: Academic Press.
  • Cruse, D.A. (1995). Lexical Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Cruse, D.A. (2000). Meaning in Language. An Introduction to Semantics and Pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Cruse, D.A. (2002). Hyponymy and its varieties. In R. Green, C.A. Bean, & S.H. Myaeng (Eds.), The Semantics of Relationships. An Interdisciplinary Perspective (pp. 3-21). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • Dunlea, A. (1989). Vision and the Emergence of Meaning. Blind and Sighted Children’s Early Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Elbers, L. & van Loon-Vervoorn, A. (1999). Lexical relationships in children who are blind. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 93 (7), 419-421.
  • Flavell, J.H., Draguns, J., Feinberg, L.D., & Budin, W. (1958). A microgenetic approach to word association. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57 (1), 1-7.
  • Forster, K.I. (2002). Accessing the mental lexicon. In G.T.M. Altmann (Ed.), Psycholinguistics. Critical Concepts in Psychology. Vol. 1 (pp. 270-296). New York: Routledge.
  • Gerstl, P. & Pribbenov, S. (1995). Midwinters, end games, and body relations: A classification of part-whole relations. International Journal of Human- Computer Studies, 43 (5-6), 865-889.
  • Grenier, D. & Giroux, N. (1997). A comparative study of spelling performance of sighted and blind students in senior high school. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 91 (4), 393-400.
  • Groenveld, M. & Jan, J.E. (1992). Intelligence profiles of low vision and blind children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 86 (1), 68-71.
  • Heller, M.A. (1991). Haptic perception in blind people. In M.A. Heller & W. Schiff(Eds.), The Psychology of Touch (pp. 239-261). Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Heller, M.A. & Schiff, W. (1991). Conclusions: the future of touch. In M.A. Heller & W. Schiff(Eds.), The Psychology of Touch (pp. 327-337). Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Houde, O. (1990). Logical categorization: schematic knowledge, categorical knowledge, and image versus linguistic format. A study in six- to eleven-year-olds. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 10 (4), 343-384.
  • Hunca-Bednarska, A. (1997). Skojarzenia werbalne w schizofrenii [Verbal Associations in Schizophrenia]. Lublin: Czelej.
  • Jaworska-Biskup, K. (2011). The world without sight. A comparative study of concept understanding in Polish congenitally totally blind and sighted children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 15 (1), 33-47.
  • Levinson, S.C. (1999). From outer to inner space: Linguistic categories and nonlinguistic thinking. In J. Nuyts & E. Pederson (Eds.), Language and Conceptualization (pp. 13-45). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Lewis, V. (2003). Development and Disability. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
  • Lyons, J. (1984). Semantyka 1 [Semantics 1]. Warszawa: PWN.
  • Mac Cluskie, K.C., Tunick, R.H., Dial, J.G., & Paul, D.S. (1998). The role of vision in the development of abstraction ability. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 92 (3), 189-199.
  • Martinez-Manrique, F. (2010). On the distinction between semantic and conceptual representation. Dialectica, 64 (1), 57-78. Mikołajczak-Matyja, N. (2004). Skojarzenia słowne niewidomych i widzących użytkownikow języka polskiego - studium porownawcze [Verbal associations of blind and sighted users of the Polish language - a comparative study]. Investigationes Linguisticae, 11, 1-17.
  • Mikołajczak-Matyja, N. (2008). Hierarchiczna struktura leksykonu umysłowego. Relacje semantyczne w leksykonie widzących i niewidomych użytkownikow języka [The Hierarchical Structure of Mental Lexicon. Semantic Relations in the Lexicon of Sighted and Blind Language Users]. Poznań: Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM. Mikołajczak-Matyja, N. (2010). The superordination relation and the symmetry of verbal associations in selected parts of the mental lexicon. Psychology of Language and Communication, 14 (2), 15-35.
  • Mikołajczak-Matyja, N. (2014). The distinction between the concept and the meaning of the word in light of theories of concepts structure and of speech processing models. Pensee Journal, 76 (7), 147-164.
  • Moss, H.E., Ostrin, R.K., Tyler, L.K., & Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (1995). Accessing different types of lexical semantic information: evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21 (4), 863-883.
  • Mulford, R. (1988). First words of the blind child. In M.D. Smith & J.L. Locke (Eds.), The Emerging Lexicon. The Child’s Development of a Linguistic Vocabulary (pp. 293-338). New York: Academic Press.
  • Murphy, M.L. (2003). Semantic Relations and the Lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Pederson, E. & Nuyts, J. (1999). Overview: On the relationship between language and conceptualization. In J. Nuyts & E. Pederson (Eds.), Language and Conceptualization (pp. 1-12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Perez-Pereira, M. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (1999). Language Development and Social Interaction in Blind Children. Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Pietrulewicz, B. (1983). Rozwoj rozumowania przez analogię u dzieci niewidomych w wieku szkolnym [Development of Reasoning by Analogy in School-Age Blind Children]. Wrocław: Ossolineum.
  • Piskorska, A. (2008). Concepts refl ecting aesthetic judgments in blind children. In E. Mioduszewska & A. Piskorska (Eds.), Relevance Round Table (pp. 97-108). Warszawa: Wydawnictwa UW.
  • Pribbenov, S. (2002). Meronymic relationships: From classical mereology to complex part-whole relations. In R. Green, C.A. Bean, & S.H. Myaeng (Eds.),The Semantics of Relationships. An Interdisciplinary Perspective (pp. 35-50). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • Roder, B., Demuth, L., Streb J., & Rosler, F. (2003). Semantic and morpho-syntactic priming in auditory word recognition in congenitally blind adults. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18 (1), 1-20.
  • Roe, J. & Webster, A. (1997). Children with Visual Impairments: Social Interaction, Language and Learning. New York: Routledge. Russell, W.A. & Meseck, O.R. (1959). Der Einfl uss der Assoziation auf das Erinnern von Worten in der deutschen, franzosischen und englischen Sprache. Zeitschrift fur Experimentale und Angewandete Psychologie, 6, 191-211.
  • Sękowska, Z. (1974). Kształcenie dzieci niewidomych [Education of Blind Children]. Warszawa: PWN.
  • Szczechowicz, A. (1976). Swoistość kształtowania się pojęć u dzieci niewidomych [The specificity of concept forming in blind children]. In K. Klimasiński (Ed.), Procesy poznawcze a defekty sensoryczne. Materiały I Krajowego Sympozjum Psychologii Defektologicznej [Cognitive Processes and Sensory Deficits. Materials of I National Symposium of Defectological Psychology] (pp. 86-99). Warszawa: Polski Związek Głuchych.
  • Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1996). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  • Tillman, M.H. & Williams, C. (1968). Associative characteristics of blind and sighted children to selected form classes. International Journal for the Education of the Blind, 18 (2), 33-40.
  • Tobin, M. (2008). Information: A new paradigm for research into our understanding of blindness? British Journal of Visual Impairment, 26 (2), 119-127.
  • Tversky, B. (1990). Where partonomies and taxonomies meet. In S.L. Tsohatzidis (Ed.), Meaning and Prototypes. Studies in Linguistic Categorization (pp. 334-344). New York: Routledge.
  • Upmanyu, V.V., Bhardwaj, S., & Singh, S. (1996). Word-association emotional indicators: Associations with anxiety, psychoticism, neuroticism, extraversion, and creativity. The Journal of Social Psychology, 136 (4), 521-529.[PubMed]
  • Warren, D.H. (1994). Blindness and Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Werth, P. (1983). Meaning in language acquisition. In A.E. Mills (Ed.), Language Acquisition in the Blind Child. Normal and Deficient (pp. 77-88). London: Croom Helm.
  • Wharton, T. (2004). Lexical acquisition and pragmatics. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 16, 323-341.
  • Wolter, B. (2002). Assessing proficiency through word associations: Is there still hope? System, 3 (3), 315-329.
  • Wyver, S.R., Markham, R., & Hlavacek, S. (1999). Visual items in tests of intelligence for children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 93 (10), 663-665.
  • Wyver, S.R., Markham, R., & Hlavacek, S. (2000). Inferences and word associations of children with visual impairments. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 94 (4), 204-217.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_1515_plc-2015-0001
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.