Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2015 | 46 | 3 | 421-432

Article title

Emotional and attentional predictors of self-regulation in early childhood

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
The development of self-regulation in early childhood is related to development of emotional regulation and attention, in particular executive attention (Feldman, 2009; Posner & Rothbart, 1998). As the ability to self-regulate is crucial in life (Casey et al., 2011), it is important to reveal early predictors of self-regulation. The aim of the paper is to present the results of longitudinal studies on the relationships between the functioning of attention, regulation of emotion and later self-regulatory abilities. 310 children were assessed at three time points. At 12 months of age emotional regulation in situation of frustration and attention regulation were assessed. At 18 and 24 months behavioral-emotional regulation in the Snack Delay Task was measured. Additionally parents assessed executive attention using The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire when children were 26 months old. Structural equation modelling revealed two different paths to development of self-regulatory abilities at 18 months: emotional (reactive system) and emotionalattentional and only one emotional-attentional path at 24 months. The early ability to focus attention and later executive attention functioning revealed to be important predictors of self-regulatory abilities both at 18 and 24 months of age.

Year

Volume

46

Issue

3

Pages

421-432

Physical description

Dates

published
2015-09-01
online
2015-11-17

Contributors

  • Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Poland
author
  • Jesuit University Ignatianum in Krakow, Poland
  • Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Poland
  • Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Poland
  • Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Poland
  • Jagiellonian University, Institute of Psychology, Poland

References

  • Ahadi, S.A., Rothbart, M.K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children’s temperament in the US and China: Similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359-377. doi: 10.1002/per.2410070506[Crossref]
  • Białek, A., Białecka-Pikul, M., & Stępień-Nycz, M. (2014). The Nature of Child-Adult Interaction. From Turn-Taking to Understanding Pointing and Use of Pointing Gestures. Psychology of Language and Communication, 18, 87-105. doi: 10.2478/plc-2014-0008.[Crossref]
  • Behne, T., Carpenter M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8, 492-499. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00440.x [Crossref]
  • Berger, A. (2011). Self-Regulation: Brain, Cognition, and Development. Human brain development series. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
  • Berger, A., Kofman, O., Livneh, U., & Henik, A. (2007). Multidisciplinary perspectives on attention and the development of self-regulation. Progress in Neurobiology, 82, 256-286. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.06.004[Crossref]
  • Braungart-Rieker, J.M., & Stifter, C.A. (1996). Infants’ responses to frustrating situations: Continuity and change in reactivity and regulation. Child Development, 67, 1767-1779. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01826.x [Crossref]
  • Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., & Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and Differentiating Aspects of Self- Regulation: Effortful Control, Executive Functioning, and Links to Negative Affectivity. Emotion, 13, 47-63. doi: 10.1037/a0029536.[Crossref]
  • Bruner, J. (1984). Interaction, communication, and self. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 23, 1-7.
  • Byczewska-Konieczny, K., Kosno, M., Stępień-Nycz, M., Białecka-Pikul, M., & Białek, A., (submitted). Waiting for a treat. Coping strategies in situations of delayed gratifi cation in 18- and 24-month-old children.
  • Calkins, S.D., Fox, N.A., & Marshall, T.R. (1996). Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibition in infancy. Child Development, 67, 523-540. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01749.x [Crossref]
  • Calkins, S.D., Smith, C.L., Gill, K.L., & Johnson, M.C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7, 350-369. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00072[Crossref]
  • Casey, B.J., Somerville, L.H., Gotlib, I.H., Ayduk, O., Franklin, N.T., Askren, M.K., Jonides, J., Berman, M.G., Wilson, N.L., Teslovich, T., Glover, G., Zayas, V., Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y., (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratifi cation 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 14998-15003. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1108561108[Crossref]
  • Dawson, P., & Guare, R. (2009). Smart but Scattered: The Revolutionary “Executive Skills” Approach to Helping Kids Reach Their Potential. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. (1988). Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 958-966. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.6.958[Crossref]
  • Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R.A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon, & N. Eisenberg (eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development (pp. 701-788). New York: Wiley.
  • Feldman, R. (2004). Mother-infant skin-to-skin contact and the development of emotion regulation. In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 27, pp. 113-131). Hauppauge, NY: Nova.
  • Feldman, R. (2009). The Development of Regulatory Functions From Birth to 5 Years: Insights From Premature Infants. Child Development, 8, 544-461. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01278.x [Crossref]
  • Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2002). Early intervention and the development of self-regulation. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 22, 307-335. doi: 10.1080/07351692209348990[Crossref]
  • Fox, N.A., Bell, M.A., & Jones, N.A. (1992). Individual differences in response to stress and cerebral asymmetry. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 161-184. doi: 10.1080/87565649209540523[Crossref]
  • Gartstein, M.A., & Rothbart, M.K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 64-86. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8[Crossref]
  • Gerardi-Caulton, G. (2000). Sensitivity to spatial confl ict and the development of self-regulation in children 24-36 months of age. Developmental Science 3, 397-404. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00134
  • Gray, J.A., & McNaughton, N. (1996). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: reprise. In D.A. Hope (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1995: Perspectives on anxiety, panic, and fear. Current theory and research in motivation (Vol. 43, pp. 61-134). Lincoln, NE, US: University of Nebraska Press.
  • Grolnick, W., Bridges, L., & Connell, J. (1996). Emotion regulation in two-year-olds: strategies and emotional expression in four contexts. Child Development, 67, 928-941. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996. tb01774.x [Crossref]
  • Harman, C., Rothbart, M.K., & Posner, M.I. (1997). Distress and attention interactions in early infancy. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 27-43.
  • Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K.R., Lee, W.S., & Zelazo, P.D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617-644. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4[Crossref]
  • Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent-child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83, 1275-1289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01778.x[Crossref]
  • Kiss, M., Fechete, G., Pop, M., & Susa, G. (2014). Early childhood selfregulation in context: parental and familial environmental infl uences. Cognition, Brain, Behavior. An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18(1), 55-85.
  • Kmita, G. (2013). Od zaciekawienia do zaangażowania. O rozwoju samoregulacji w interakcjach z rodzicami niemowląt urodzonych skrajnie wcześnie, przedwcześnie i o czasie [From interest to committment. On the development of self-regulation in interactions with parents of extremely premature, premature and mature infants]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego.
  • Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mothers’ discipline, and security of attachment: multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597-615. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00892.x[Crossref]
  • Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68, 94-112. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997. tb01928.x [Crossref]
  • Kochanska, G., Coy, K.C., & Murray, K.T. (2001). The Development of Self-Regulation in the First Four Years of Life. Child Development, 72, 1091-1111. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00336[Crossref]
  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K.T., & Harlan, E. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220-232. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220[Crossref]
  • Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199-214. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.199[Crossref]
  • Mischel, W. (2012). Self-control theory. In P.A.M. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 1-22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2003). Sustaining delay of gratifi cation over time: A hot/cool systems perspective. In G. Loewenstein, D. Read, & R. Baumeister (Eds.), Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice (pp. 175-200). New York: Russell Sage.
  • Mischel, W., Shoda Y., & Rodriguez M.L. (1989). Delay of gratifi cation in children. Science, 244, 933-938. doi: 10.1126/science.2658056[Crossref]
  • Mittal, R.; Russell, B., Britner, P., & Peake, P. (2013). Delay of Gratifi cation in Two- and Three-Year-Olds: Associations with Attachment, Personality, and Temperament. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 22, 479-489. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9600-6[Crossref]
  • Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the Abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, Florida: University of Miami Psychology Department.
  • Peake, P.K., Hebl, M., & Mischel, W. (2002). Strategic attention deployment for delay of gratifi cation in working and waiting situations. Developmental Psychology, 38, 313-326. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.313.[Crossref]
  • Posner, M.I., & Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends in Neuroscience, 17, 75-79. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7[Crossref]
  • Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (1998). Developing attentional skills. In J. Richards (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention: A developmental perspective. (pp. 317-323), Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of selfregulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427-441. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400003096[Crossref]
  • Putnam, S.P., Gartstein, M.A., & Rothbart, M.K. (2006). Measurement of fi ne-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 386-401. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004[Crossref]
  • Putnam, S.P., Rothbart, M.K., & Gartstein, M.A. (2008). Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fi ne-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood. Infant and Child Development, 17, 387-405. doi: 10.1002/icd.582[Crossref]
  • Rothbart, M., Ahadi, S., & Hershey, K. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 40, 21-39.
  • Rothbart, M.K., Ahadi, S.A., Hershey, K.L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 1394-1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355.[Crossref]
  • Rothbart, M.K., & Bates, J.E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105-176). New York: Wiley.
  • Rothbart, M.K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In M.E. Lamb, & A.L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 37-86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Rothbart, M.K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M.I. (1994). A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament. In J.E. Bates, & T.D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual Differences at the Interface of Biology and Behavior (pp. 83-116), Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
  • Rothbart, M.K., Ellis, L.K., & Posner, M.I. (2004). Temperament and selfregulation. In R.F. Baumeister, & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self- regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 357-370). New York: Guilford.
  • Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 573-594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2[Crossref]
  • Sethi, A., Mischel, W., Aber, J.L., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M.L. (2000). The role of strategic attention deployment in development of selfregulation: Predicting preschoolers’ delay of gratifi cation from mother- toddler interactions. Developmental Psychology, 36, 767-777. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.6.767[Crossref]
  • Silverman, I. W., & Ippolito, M. F. (1995). Maternal antecedents at delay ability in young children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 569-591. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(95)90005-5[Crossref]
  • Stępień-Nycz, M., Białecka-Pikul, M., Białek, A., Rostek, I., & Baran, M. (2013). Emotional and attentional aspects of self-regulation in infancy. Poster presented at the XVIth European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Lausanne, Switzerland.
  • Stifter, C.A., & Braungart, J.M. (1995). The regulation of negative reactivity: Function and development. Developmental Psychology, 38, 448-455. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.3.448[Crossref]
  • Stifter, C.A., Spinrad, T.L., & Braungart-Rieker, J.M. (1999). Toward a developmental model of child compliance: the role of emotion regulation in infancy. Child Development, 70, 21-32. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00003[Crossref]
  • Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
  • Thompson, R.A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 269-307. doi: 10.1007/ BF01319934[Crossref]
  • Van der Mark, I., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & Van Ijzendoorn, M.H. (2002). The role of parenting, attachment, and temperamental fearfulness in the prediction of compliance in toddler girls. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 361-378. doi: 10.1348/026151002320620299[Crossref]
  • Vaughn, B.E., Kopp, Clair B., & Krakow, J.B. (1984). The Emergence and Consolidation of Self-Control from Eighteen to Thirty Months of Age: Normative Trends and Individual Differences. Child Development, 55, 990-1004. doi: 10.2307/1130151[Crossref]
  • Yates, G.C., Yates, S.M., & Beasley, C J. (1987). Young children’s knowledge of strategies in delay of gratification. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 33, 159-169.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_1515_ppb-2015-0049
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.