Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2016 | 47 | 1 | 12-20

Article title

Task conditions and short-term memory search: two-phase model of STM search

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
Short-term memory (STM) search, as investigated within the Sternberg paradigm, is usually described as exhaustive rather than self-terminated, although the debate concerning these issues is still hot. We report three experiments employing a modified Sternberg paradigm and show that whether STM search is exhaustive or self-terminated depends on task conditions. Specifically, STM search self-terminates as soon as a positive match is found, whereas exhaustive search occurs when the STM content does not contain a searched item. Additionally, we show that task conditions influence whether familiarity- or recollection-based strategies dominate STM search performance. Namely, when speeding up the tempo of stimuli presentation increases the task demands, people use familiarity-based retrieval more often, which results in faster but less accurate recognition judgments. We conclude that STM search processes flexibly adapt to current task conditions and finally propose two-phase model of STM search.

Year

Volume

47

Issue

1

Pages

12-20

Physical description

Dates

published
2016-04-01
online
2016-05-14

Contributors

  • Jagiellonian University, Poland
author
  • Jagiellonian University, Poland
author
  • Polish Academy of Sciences, Pola

References

  • Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 83-100.
  • Cave, K. R., Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 225-271.
  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450-466.
  • Gilchrist, Cowan (2014). A two-stage search of visual working memory: investigating speed in the change-detection paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(7), 2031-2050.[WoS]
  • Göthe, K., & Oberauer, K. (2008). The integration of familiarity and recollection information in short-term recognition: modeling speedaccuracy trade-off functions. Psychological Research, 72, 289-303.[WoS]
  • Haygood, R. C., & Johnson, D. F. (1983). Focus shift and individual differences in the Sternberg memory-search task. Acta Psychologica, 53, 129-139.[WoS]
  • Hertzog, Ch., Cooper, B. P., & Fisk, A. D. (1996). Aging and individual differences in the development of skilled memory search performance. Psychology and Aging, 11, 497-520.
  • Hunt, E. B. (1980). Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449-474.
  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513-541.
  • Jensen, A. R. (1987). Process differences and individual differences in some cognitive tasks. Intelligence, 11, 107-136.
  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122-149.
  • Logan, G. D. (2002). An instance theory of attention and memory. Psychological Review, 109, 376-400.
  • Nęcka, E. (2000). Pobudzenie intelektu. Zarys formalnej teorii inteligencji. (Arousal of intellect: An outline of the formal theory of intelligence). Kraków, Poland: Universitas.
  • Oberauer, K. (2001). Removing irrelevant information from working memory: A cognitive aging study with the modified Sternberg task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 948-957.
  • Schneider, W. X. (1999). Visual-spatial working memory, attention, and scene representation: A neuro-cognitive theory. Psychological Research, 62, 220-236.
  • Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652-654.
  • Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory scanning: mental processes revealed by time experiments. American Scientist, 57, 17-18.
  • Sternberg, S. (1975). Memory scanning: New findings and current controversies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 1-32.[Crossref]
  • Townsend, J. T., & Colonius, H. (1997). Parallel processing response times and experimental determination of the stopping rule. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 41, 392-397.
  • Townsend, J. T., & Fific, M. (2004). Parallel versus serial processing and individual differences in high-speed search in human memory. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 953-962.
  • Townsend, J. T., & Van Zandt, T. (1990). New theoretical results on testing self-terminating vs exhaustive processing in rapid search experiments. In: Geissler, H. G., Miller, Martin H., Prinz, W. (Eds.) Psychophysical explorations of mental structures (pp. 469-489). Ashland, OH, US: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.
  • Treisman, A. M., & Gromican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15-48.
  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.
  • Van Zandt, T., & Townsend, J. T. (1993). Self-terminating versus exhaustive processes in rapid visual and memory search: An evaluative review. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 563-580.
  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0. A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 202-238.
  • Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 419-433.[WoS]
  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601-621.[WoS]
  • Yonelinas, A. P. (1997). Recognition memory ROCs for item and associative information: The contribution of recollection and familiarity. Memory & Cognition, 25, 747-763.
  • Yonelinas, A. P. (1999). The contribution of recollection and familiarity to recognition and source-memory judgments: A formal dual-process model and an analysis of receiver operating characteristics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 1415-1434.[WoS]
  • Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441-517.[WoS]
  • Yonelinas, A. P., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Dissociations of processes in recognition memory: Effects of interference and of response speed. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 516-534.
  • Yonelinas, A. P., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Noncriterial recollection: Familiarity as automatic, irrelevant recollection. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 131-141.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_1515_ppb-2016-0002
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.