Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2016 | 47 | 1 | 92-102

Article title

Where was it? Working memory as a predictor of passive vocabulary development in the third year of life

Content

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
The objective of the presented research was to test whether working memory, measured using the Spin the Pots task, is an important factor in passive vocabulary development in 2- and 3-year-old children. Two longitudinal studies were conducted. In the first, 135 children participated in the first study. At 18 months their responding to joint attention was measured, and then at 24 months their working memory and passive vocabulary was tested. It was demonstrated that responding to joint attention predicts the level of development of working memory, which in turn influences the extent of the passive vocabulary. In the second study, 113 children participated - at 30 months their working memory was measured, and at 24 months and then 36 months their passive vocabulary was tested. It was observed that at the age of 3 working memory is still a significant predictor of passive vocabulary. The usefulness of the Spin the Pots task for measuring the working memory of young children was shown and discussed in conclusions.

Year

Volume

47

Issue

1

Pages

92-102

Physical description

Dates

published
2016-04-01
online
2016-05-14

Contributors

  • Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University
  • Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University
  • Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University

References

  • Adams, A. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38(2), 403-414. DOI:10.1044/jshr.3802.403[Crossref]
  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., & Willis, C. (2005). Working memory abilities in children with special educational needs. Educational and Child Psychology, 22, 56-67.
  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. M. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(2), 85-106. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.002[Crossref]
  • Awh, E., Vogel, E. K., & Oh, S. H. (2006). Interactions between attention and working memory. Neuroscience, 139(1), 201-208. DOI:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.023[Crossref]
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423. DOI:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2[Crossref]
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1-29. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422[Crossref]
  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In: G. A. Bower (Ed.) The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 8. (pp. 47-89). New York: Academic Press.
  • Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158-173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037
  • Bakeman, R. & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development, 55 (4), 1278-1289.[Crossref]
  • Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In: Ch. Moore, P. J. Dunham (Eds.) Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, 131-158. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Barrett, M., Harris, M., & Chasin, J. (1991). Early lexical development and maternal speech: A comparison of children’s initial and subsequent uses of words. Journal of Child Language, 18(01), 21-40. DOI: 10.1017/S0305000900013271[Crossref]
  • Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 83-100. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.83[Crossref]
  • Białek, A., Białecka-Pikul, M., & Stępień-Nycz, M. (2014). The Nature of Child-Adult Interaction. From Turn-Taking to Understanding Pointing and Use of Pointing Gestures. Psychology of Language and Communication, 18(2), 87-105. DOI: 10.2478/plc-2014-0008[Crossref]
  • Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 44-62. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.44[Crossref]
  • Blake, J., Austin, W., Cannon, M., Lisus, A., & Vaughan, A. (1994). The relationship between memory span and measures of imitative and spontaneous language complexity in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17(1), 91-107. DOI: 10.1177/016502549401700106[Crossref]
  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(01), 207-220. DOI: 10.1017/S030500090700829X [Crossref]
  • Carlson, S., M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children, Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 2, 595-616. DOI:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3[Crossref]
  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), i-174. DOI: 10.2307/1166214[Crossref]
  • Conklin, H. M., Luciana, M., Hooper, C. J., & Yarger, R. S. (2007). Working memory performance in typically developing children and adolescents: Behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31(1), 103-128. DOI: 10.1080/87565640709336889[Crossref]
  • Courage, M. L., & Cowan, N. (Eds.). (2009). The development of memory in infancy and childhood. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  • Cowan, N. (2011). The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks: making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1401-1406. DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia. 2011.01.035[Crossref]
  • Cuevas, K., & Bell, M. A. (2014). Infant attention and early childhood executive function. Child Development, 85(2), 397-404. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12126[Crossref]
  • D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., Zarahn, E., Ballard, D., Shin, R. K., & Lease, J. (1998). Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 7(1), 1-13. DOI:10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00004-4[Crossref]
  • Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Development, 56(4), 868-883. DOI: 10.2307/1130099[Crossref]
  • Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. Stuss, R. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466-503). London, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234-248. DOI: 10.1111/desc.12019[Crossref]
  • Fougnie, D. (2008). The relationship between attention and working memory. In N. B. Johansen (Ed.), New research on short-term memory, (pp 1-45). Nova Science Publishers.
  • Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (2000). Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Biological Psychology, 54(1), 1-34. DOI:10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00051-X [Crossref]
  • Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rulebased reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10(4), 483-527. DOI:10.1016/0885-2014(95)90024-1[Crossref]
  • Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(04), 513-543. DOI: 10.1017/S0142716406060383[Crossref]
  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 200-213. DOI:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2[Crossref]
  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition?. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8(3), 259-272. DOI: 10.1007/BF03174081[Crossref]
  • Gathercole, S. E., Lamont, E. M. I. L. Y., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Working memory in the classroom. In: S. J. Pickering (Ed.) Working memory and education (pp. 219-240), Oxford: Elsevier.
  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177-190. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177[Crossref]
  • Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 887-898. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.887[Crossref]
  • Gauvain, M. (2013). Sociocultural context of development. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The oxford handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 425-451). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Haman, E., Fronczyk, K. (2012). Obrazkowy Test Słownikowy - Rozumienie. Gdańsk: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych i Pedagogicznych.
  • Henry, L. (2012). The Development of Working Memory in Children, London: SAGE Publications Ltd.
  • Hick, R. F., Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Short-term memory and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 47(8), 532-538. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01187.x [Crossref]
  • Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368-1378. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612[Crossref]
  • Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73(2), 418-433. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00415[Crossref]
  • Hornowska, E. (2005). Testy psychologiczne. Teoria i praktyka. Warszawa: wyd. Scholar.
  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 645-668. DOI:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5[Crossref]
  • Jarrold, C., Thorn, A. S., & Stephens, E. (2009). The relationships among verbal short-term memory, phonological awareness, and new word learning: Evidence from typical development and Down syndrome. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(2), 196-218. DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.001[Crossref]
  • Kastner, J. W., May, W., & Hildman, L. (2001). Relationship between language skills and academic achievement in first grade. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92(2), 381-390. DOI: 10.2466/pms.2001.92.2.381[Crossref]
  • Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6(5), 449-467. DOI: 10.1111/1467-7687.00300[Crossref]
  • Kurcz, I. (2005). Psychologia języka i komunikacji. Warszawa: Wyd. Scholar.
  • Lee, J. (2011). Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(1), 69-92. DOI: 10.1017/S0142716410000299[Crossref]
  • Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., Hooper, C. J., & Yarger, R. S. (2005). The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development, 76(3), 697-712. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00872.x [Crossref]
  • Müller, U., Jacques, S., Brocki, K., & Zelazo, P. D. (2009). The executive functions of language in preschool children. In A. Winsler, Ch. Fernyhough, & I. Montero (Eds.). Private Speech, Executive Functioning, and the Development of Verbal Self-Regulation (pp. 53-68). Cambridge University Press.
  • Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2015). Examining executive function in the second year of life: Coherence, stability, and relations to joint attention and language. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 101-114. DOI: 10.1037/a0038359[Crossref]
  • Montgomery, J. W., & Evans, J. L. (2009). Complex sentence comprehension and working memory in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 269-288. DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388[Crossref]
  • Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 373-377. DOI:10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90014-5
  • Mundy, P., & Brunette, C. (2005). Joint attention and neurodevelopment. In: E. Volkmar, A. Klin, & R. Paul (Eds.). Handbook of autism and persvasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 650-681). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
  • Mundy, P. (2013). A neural networks, information-processing model of joint attention and social-cognitive development. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology (pp. 217-241). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of infant joint attention. Child Development, 78 (3), 938-954. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x [Crossref]
  • Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.
  • Newcombe, N. S. (2011). What Is Neoconstructivism? Child Development Perspectives, 5, 157-160. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00180.x [Crossref]
  • Olson, D. R. (1993). The development of representations. The origins of mental life. Canadian Psychology, 34, 293-306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0078834
  • Orzechowski, J. (2012). Magiczna liczba jeden, czyli co jeszcze zmieści się w pamięci roboczej. Kraków: WUJ.
  • Orzechowski, J., Piotrowski, K. T., Balas, R., Stettner, Z. (red.) (2009). Pamięć robocza. Warszawa: Academica.
  • Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554-566. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554[Crossref]
  • Pham, A. V., & Hasson, R. M. (2014). Verbal and visuospatial working memory as predictors of children’s reading ability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, DOI:10.1093/arclin/acu024[Crossref]
  • Pungello, E. P., Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., Mills-Koonce, R., & Reznick, J. S. (2009). The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 544-557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013917
  • Reznick, J. S. (2009). Working memory in infant and toddlers. In M. L. Courage & Cowan (Eds.). The development of memory in infancy and childhood (pp. 343-365). Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Rheingold, H., Hey, D., & West, M. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development, 47 (4), 1148-1158. DOI: 10.2307/1128454[Crossref]
  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.
  • Savage, R., Cornish, K., Manly, T., & Hollis, C. (2006). Cognitive processes in children’s reading and attention: The role of working memory, divided attention, and response inhibition. British Journal of Psychology, 97(3), 365-385. DOI: 10.1348/000712605X81370[Crossref]
  • Scofield, J., & Behrend, D. A. (2011). Clarifying the role of joint attention in early word learning. First Language, 31, 326-341, DOI: 10.1177/0142723710395423[Crossref]
  • Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The early social-communication scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3 (4), 244-258. DOI: 10.1002/1097 -0355(198224)3:4<244::AID-IMHJ2280030406>3.0.CO;2-R [Crossref]
  • Sigman, M., & McGovern, C. W. (2005). Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 15-23. DOI: 10.1007/ s10803-004-1027-5[Crossref]
  • Stokes, S. F., & Klee, T. (2009). Factors that influence vocabulary development in two-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), 498-505. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01991.x [Crossref]
  • Swanson, H. L. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled readers’ working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 1-31. DOI:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00043-2[Crossref]
  • Swanson, H. L. (2006). Working memory and reading disabilities: Both phonological and executive processing deficits are important. In: T. P. Alloway & S. E. Gathercole (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 59-88). Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(4), 381-398. DOI: 10.1207/s1532799xssr1004_3[Crossref]
  • Tomasello, M. (1988). The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences, 10(1), 69-88. DOI:10.1016/0388-0001(88)90006-X [Crossref]
  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In: Ch. Moore, P. J. Dunham (Eds.) Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, (pp. 103-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(6), 1454-1463. DOI: 10.2307/1130423[Crossref]
  • Van Hecke, A. V., Mundy, P., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E., Parlade, M. V., Pomares, Y. B., & Hobson, J. A. (2012). Infant responding to joint attention, executive processes, and self-regulation in preschool children. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(2), 303-311. DOI:10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.12.001[Crossref]
  • Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children IVUK. London; Psychological Corporation
  • Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2007). Sources of variability in working memory in early childhood: A consideration of age, temperament, language, and brain electrical activity. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 431-455. DOI:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.007[Crossref]
  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11(1), 37-63. DOI:10.1016/S0885-2014(96)90027-1[Crossref]

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_1515_ppb-2016-0010
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.