Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


2014 | 18 | 3 | 226-250

Article title

Switching Between Noun and Verb Agreement Rules Comes at a Cost: Cross-Sectional and Interventional Studies in a Developmental Sample

Title variants

Languages of publication

EN

Abstracts

EN
This study clarifies the impact of switching context between noun and verb number agreement rules in written language production. In Experiment 1, children from grade 3 to 6 were asked to fill in sentences with nouns and verbs in either a switching condition (noun followed by verb) or a repeating condition (noun followed by noun). The results showed that third- and fourth-grade children produced more erroneous agreements in the switching condition than in the repeating condition, showing that switching between rules comes at a cost, whereas fifth- and sixth-grade participants’ performance was not affected by the switching context. Based on these findings, Experiment 2 aimed to assess whether a switching treatment offers a greater opportunity to improve the acquisition of grammatical agreement production, as compared to a simple treatment. Teachers from grade 3 gave either a switching treatment (mixed noun and verb exercises) or a simple treatment (noun exercises followed by verb exercises). The results show that children learned better from the switching treatment than from the simple treatment. These findings highlight the cost of switching between noun and verb agreement rules during the acquisition of grammatical number agreement and also how grammatical spelling acquisition can be improved at school.

Publisher

Year

Volume

18

Issue

3

Pages

226-250

Physical description

Dates

published
2014-11-01
online
2014-12-30

Contributors

  • Université Catholique de Louvain
  • Université Catholique de Louvain
author
  • Université Catholique de Louvain
  • Université Catholique de Louvain

References

  • Alamargot, D., Caporossi, G., Chesnet, D., & Ros, C. (2011). What makes a skilled writer? Working memory and audience awareness during text composition. Learning and Individual Differences, 21 (5), 505-516.[Crossref][WoS]
  • Alamargot, D., Flouret, L., Larocque, D., Caporossi, G., Pontart, V., Paduraru, C., Morisset, P., & Fayol, M. (2014). Successful written subject-verb agreement: an online analysis of the procedure used by students in Grades 3, 5 and 12. Reading and Writing.
  • Anderson, J.R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89 (4), 369-406.[Crossref]
  • Anderson, J.R. (1996). ACT - A simple theory of complex cognition. American Psychologist, 51 (4), 355-365.[Crossref]
  • Baayen, R.H., Davidson, D.J., & Bates, D.M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (4), 390-412.[WoS]
  • Beers, C.S. & Beers, J. W. (1992). Children’s spelling of English inflectional morphology. In S. Templeton & D.R. Bear (Eds.), Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy (pp. 231-251). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Cepeda, N.J., Kramer, A.F., & de Sather, J. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37 (5), 715-730.[Crossref]
  • Chevrie-Muller, C., Simon, A.-M., & Fournier, S. (1997). Batterie “Langage oral, langage écrit, mémoire et attention” . Paris: ECPA.
  • Crone, E.A., Bunge, S.A., van der Molen, M.W., & Ridderinkhof, K.R. (2006). Switching between tasks and responses: a developmental study. Developmental Science, 9 (3), 278-287.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Crone, E.A., Somsen, R.J.M., Zanolie, K., & Van der Molen, M.W. (2006). A heart rate analysis of developmental change in feedback processing and rule shifting from childhood to early adulthood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95 (2), 99-116.[Crossref]
  • Davidson, M.C., Amso, D., Anderson, L.C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44 (11), 2037-2078.
  • Dibbets, P. & Jolles, J. (2006). The Switch Task for children: Measuring mental flexibility in young children. Cognitive Development, 21 (1), 60-71.[Crossref]
  • Ellefson, M.R., Shapiro, L.R., & Chater, N. (2006). Asymmetrical switch costs in children. Cognitive Development, 21 (2), 108-130.[Crossref]
  • Fayol, M., Hupet, M., & Largy, P. (1999). The acquisition of subject-verb agreement in written french: from novices to experts. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinarity Journal, 11 (2), 153-174.
  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K.E. Patterson, J.C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface Dyslexia: Cognitive and Neuropsychological Studies of Phonological Reading (pp. 301-330). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Kirkham, N.Z., Cruess, L., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6 (5), 449-467.[Crossref]
  • Largy, P. (2001). La révision des accords nominal et verbal chez l’enfant. L’année psychologique, 101 (2), 221-245.
  • Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). Manulex: a web-accessible lexical database from French primary school reading. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36 (1), 156-166.[Crossref]
  • Liefooghe, B., Barrouillet, P., Vandierendonck, A., & Camos, V. (2008). Working memory costs of task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (3), 478-494.
  • Lobrot, M. (1967). Batterie pour mesurer la lecture et l’orthographe. ORLEC. Beaumont/ Oise: Bureau d’études et de recherches.
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41 (1), 49-100.[Crossref][PubMed]
  • Nadeau, M. (1995). Propositions pour améliorer le transfert des connaissances en orthographe grammaticale. Québec français, 99, 35-38.
  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997a). Learning to spell regular and irregular verbs. Reading and Writing, 9 (5-6), 427-449.[Crossref]
  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997b). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33 (4), 637-649.[PubMed][Crossref]
  • Pothier, B. & Pothier, P. (2003). Echelle d’acquisition en orthographe lexicale. Paris: Retz.
  • Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4 (4), 295-312.[Crossref]
  • Totereau, C., Fayol, M., & Barrouillet, P. (1998). Overgeneralizations of number inflections in the learning of written French: the case of noun and verb. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16 (4), 447-464. [Crossref]
  • Totereau, C, Thevenin, M.G., & Fayol, M. (1997). The development of the understanding of number morphology in written French. In C.A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to Spell (pp. 97-114). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17 (2), 147-177.[Crossref]
  • Van Reybroeck, M. & Hupet, M. (2009). Acquisition of number agreement: Effects of processing demands. Journal of Writing Research, 1 (2), 153-172.
  • Van Reybroeck, M., Penneman, J., Vidick, C., & Galand, B. (in press). Orthographe grammaticale: Effet de trois interventions manipulant le coût cognitif, l’autoévaluation et le feedback sur la performance et le sentiment d’efficacité personnelle. In L. Mottier Lopez & W. Tessaro (Eds.), Le jugement professionnel, au coeur de l’évaluation et de la régulation des apprentissage. Bern: Peter Lang.
  • Vandierendonck, A. (2013). On the role of dissipating inhibition in task switching. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, 221 (1), 61-62.[WoS][Crossref]
  • Yehene, E. & Meiran, N. (2007). Is there a general task switching ability? Acta Psychologica, 126 (3), 169-195. [Crossref]

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.doi-10_2478_plc-2014-0016
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.