Full-text resources of CEJSH and other databases are now available in the new Library of Science.
Visit https://bibliotekanauki.pl

PL EN


Journal

2021 | 50 | 1 | 143-156

Article title

Zastosowanie piosenki we wspomaganiu rozwoju sprawności językowych, komunikacyjnych i poznawczych dziecka

Authors

Content

Title variants

EN
The Use of Songs to Support the Development of the Child’s Language, Communication and Cognitive Skills

Languages of publication

Abstracts

EN
A song is a frequent form of the child’s musical activity. The child’s singing fulfills many important functions: it introduces him/her into the world of music, helps establish and maintain relationships with parents/caregivers and peers, acquaints the child with many thematic areas, helps understand different social phenomena, and improves the speech apparatus. The use of songs also supports the development of other functions determining the development of speech and language. The present paper shows the functions of the child’s language, communication and cognitive, functioning that can be developed by use of children’s songs, both during daily and prevention or therapy activities.
PL
Piosenka jest częstą formą aktywności muzycznej dziecka. Dziecięce śpiewanie spełnia wiele ważnych funkcji: wprowadza w świat muzyki, pomaga nawiązywać i podtrzymywać relacje z opiekunami i rówieśnikami, zapoznaje dziecko z licznymi obszarami tematycznymi, pomaga w zrozumieniu różnych zjawisk społecznych, usprawnia aparat mowy. Wykorzystanie piosenki wspiera także rozwój innych funkcji, warunkujących rozwój mowy i języka. W niniejszym artykule zostały wskazane funkcje językowego, komunikacyjnego i poznawczego funkcjonowania dziecka, które mogą być rozwijane przez zastosowanie dziecięcej piosenki, zarówno podczas codziennych aktywności, jak i w czynnościach profilaktycznych czy terapeutycznych.

Journal

Year

Volume

50

Issue

1

Pages

143-156

Physical description

Dates

published
2021

Contributors

author
  • Uniwersytet Marii Curie-Skłodowskiej w Lublinie Katedra Logopedii i Językoznawstwa Stosowanego

References

  • Altenmüller E.O., 2001, How many music centers are in the brain?, „Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences” 930(1), s. 273–280, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05738.x.
  • Brandt A., Gebrian M., Slevc l.R., 2012, Music and early language acquisition, „Frontiers in Psychology” 3, s. 1–17, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00327.
  • Brown S., 2001, Are music and language homologues?, „Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences” 930, s. 372–374, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05745.x.
  • Calvert S.l., R.l. Billingsley R.l., 1998, Young children’s recitation of information presented by songs, „Journal of Applied. Developmental Psychology” 19, s. 97–108, doi: 10.1016/s01933973(99)80030-6.
  • Conway C.M., Christiansen M.H., 2005, Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences, „Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition” 31, s. 24–39, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.24.
  • Conway C.M., Pisoni D.B., Kronenberger W.G., 2009, The importance of sound for cognitive sequencing abilities: the auditory scaffolding hypothesis, „Current Directions in Psychological Science” 18, s. 275–279, doi; 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01651.x.
  • Conway C.M., Pisoni D.B., Anaya E.M., Karpicke J., Henning S.C., 2011, Implicit sequence learning in deaf children with cochlear implants, „Developmental Science” 14, s. 69–82, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00960.x.
  • Corbeil M., Trehub S.E., Peretz I., 2013, Speech vs. singing: infants choose happier sounds, „Frontiers in Psychology” 26, s. 1–11, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00372.
  • Corriveau K., Pasquini E., Goswami U., 2007, Basic auditory processing skills and specific language impairment: A new look at an old hypothesis, „Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research” 50(3), s. 647–66, doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/046).
  • Crowder R.G., Serafine M.l., Repp B., 1990, Physical interaction and association by contiguity in memory for the words and melodies of songs, „Memory and Cognition” 18, s. 469–476, doi: 10.3758/Bf03198480.
  • Dissanayake E., 2009, Root, leaf, blossom, or bole: Concerning the origin and adaptive function of music, [w:] Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, red. S. Malloch, C. Trevarthen, Oxford, s. 17–30.
  • Ellis N., Collins l., 2009, Input and second language acquisition: The roles of frequency, form, and function introduction to the special issue, „The Modern Language Journal 93(3), s. 329–335, doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2009.00893.x.
  • Elman J.L., 1993, Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small, „Cognition” 48, s. 71–99, doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90058-4.
  • Fedorenko E., Patel A., Casasanto D., Winawer J., Gibson E., 2009, Structural integration in language and music: Evidence for a shared system, „Memory & Cognition” 37(1), s. 1–9, doi: 10.3758/mC.37.1.1.
  • Gogate l.J., Bahrick l., 1998, Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in seven–month–old infants, „Journal of Experimental Child Psychology” 69, s. 133–149, doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2438.
  • Henshaw F., 2012, How effective are affective activities? Relative benefits of two types of structured input activities as part of a computer-delivered lesson on the Spanish subjunctive, „Language Teaching Research” 16(3), s. 393–414, doi: 10.1177/1362168812436919.
  • Jentschke S., Koelsch, S., 2009, Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children, „Neuroimage” 47, s. 735–744, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.090.
  • Johnson E.K., Jusczyk P.W., 2001, Word segmentation by 8-month-olds: When speech cues count more than statistics, „Journal of Memory and Language” 44(4), s. 548–567, doi: 10.1006/ jmla.2000.2755.
  • Jusczyk P.W., Aslin R.N., 1995, Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech, „Cognitive Psychology” 29, s. 1–23, doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1010.
  • Juslin P., Laukka P., 2003, Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: different channels, same code, „Psychological Bulletin” 129, s. 770–814, doi: 10.1037/00332909.129.5.770.
  • Koelsch S., Siebel W.A., 2005, Towards a neural basis of music perception, „Trends in Cognitive Science” 9(12), s. 578–584, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.001.
  • Kotilahti K., Nissilä I., Näsi T., Lipiäinen l., Noponen T., Meriläinen P., Huotilainen M., Vineta Fellman V., 2010, Hemodynamic responses to speech and music in newborn infants, „Human Brain Mapping” 31(4), s. 595–603, doi: 10.1002/hbm.20890.
  • Kuhl P., 2004, Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code, „Nature Reviews. Neuroscience” 5(11), s. 831–843, doi: 10.1038/nrn1533.
  • Kultti A., 2013, Singing as language learning activity in multilingual toddler groups in preschool, „Early Child Development and Care” 183(12), s. 1955–1969, doi: 10.1080/03004430.2013. 765868.
  • Ludke K.M., Ferreira F., Overy K., 2014, Singing can facilitate foreign language learning, „Memory and Cognition”, 42(1), s. 41–52, doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0342-5.
  • Malloch S., 1999, Mother and infants and communicative musicality, „Musicae Scientiae” 3(1), s. 29–57, doi: 10.1177/10298649000030s104.
  • Maye J., Werker J.F., Gerken l., 2002, Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination, „Cognition” 82 (3), B101–B111, doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00157-3.
  • Montgomery A.P., 2012, Building literacy relationships with song-based picture books, „Canadian Music Educator 54(2), s. 41–44.
  • Newport E.l., 1990, Maturational constraints on language learning, Cognitive science” 14, s. 11–28, doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2.
  • Patel A.D, 2011, Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of speech? The OPERA hypothesis, „Frontiers in Psychology” 2, s. 142, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00142.
  • Peretz I., 2006, The nature of music from a biological perspective, „Cognition” 100(1), s. 1–32, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.004.
  • Peretz I., Radeau M., Arguin M., 2004, Two–way interactions between music and language: evidence from priming recognition of tune and lyrics in familiar songs, „Memory and Cognition” 32, s.142–152, doi: 10.3758/bf03195827.
  • Pinker S., Fodor, J., 2005, So how does the mind work?, „Mind and Language”, 20(1), s. 1–24, doi: 10.1111/j.0268-1064.2005.00274.x.
  • Sacks O., 2009, Muzykofilia: powieści o muzyce i mózgu, Poznań.
  • Saffran J.R., Newport E.l., Aslin R.N., 1996, Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues, „Journal of Memory and Language”, 35(4), s. 606–621, doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0032.
  • Saffran J.R., Johnson E.K., Aslin R.N., Newport E.l., 1999, Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults, „Cognition” 70(1), s. 27–52, doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00075-4.
  • Schön D., Boyer M., Moreno S., Besson M., Peretz I., Kolinsky R., 2008, Songs as an aid for language acquisition, „Cognition” 106, s. 975–983, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.005.
  • Schön, D., Gordon R., Campagne A., Magne C., Astésano C., Anton J.-l., Besson M., 2010, Similar cerebral networks in language, music and song perception, „NeuroImage” 51(1), s. 450–461, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.023.
  • Sundberg R., Cardoso W., 2019, Learning French through music: the development of the Bande à Part app, „Computer Assisted Language Learning”, 32 (1–2), s. 49–70, doi: 10.1080/09588221.2018.1472616.
  • Thiessen E.D., Saffran J.R., 2009, How the melody facilitates the message and vice versa in infant learning and memory, „Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences” 1169, s. 225–233, doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04547.x.
  • Tillmann B., McAdams s., 2004, Implicit learning of musical timbre sequences: statistical regularities confronted with acoustical (dis)similarities. „Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and. Cognition” 30, s. 1131–1142, doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.5.1131.
  • Trehub S.E., 2006, Infants as musical connoisseurs, [w:] The child as musician – A handbook of musical development, red. G.E. McPherson, Oxford, s. 33–49, doi: 10.1093/acprof: oso/9780198530329.003.0002.
  • Trevarthen C., 2008, The musical art of infant conversation: Narrating in the time of sympathetic experience, without rational interpretation, before words, „Musicae Scientiae”, 12(1), special Issue, s. 15–46, doi: 10.1177/1029864908012001021.
  • Trevarthen C., 2011, What young children give to their learning, making education work to sustain a community and its culture, „European Early Childhood Education Research Journal” 19(2), s. 173–193, doi: 10.1080/1350293X.2011.574405.
  • Walton P., 2014, Using singing and movement to teach pre-reading skills and word reading to kindergarten children: an exploratory study, „Language and Literacy 16(3), s. 54–77, doi: 10.20360/g2k88j.
  • Wilson E.O., 2012, The social conquest of the Earth, New York.
  • Winters K-l., 2013, Beyond words. Using the arts to enhance early reading comprehension, Birmingham.
  • Winters K-l., Griffin S., 2014, Singing is a Celebration of Language: Using Music to Enhance Young Children’s Vocabularies, „Language and Literacy 16, (3), Special Issue, s. 78–91, doi: 10.20360/g2Zk5X.
  • Wysocka M., 2019, Możliwości zastosowania muzyki w terapii logopedycznej. Przegląd badań, „Logopedia”, 48 (1), s. 215–229, doi: 10.24335/fqr2-tg45.
  • Yang C., 2004, Universal grammar, statistics or both?, „Trends in Cognitive Sciences” 8(10), s. 451–456, doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.006.

Document Type

Publication order reference

Identifiers

Biblioteka Nauki
1954810

YADDA identifier

bwmeta1.element.ojs-doi-10_24335_att7-6b30
JavaScript is turned off in your web browser. Turn it on to take full advantage of this site, then refresh the page.